36 research outputs found

    A neural basis for unique hues?

    Get PDF
    SummaryThe four perceptually simple colors — red, green, yellow and blue — are a challenge to neuroscience, because no one has found cortical cells that represent color in terms of these ‘unique hues’ [1]. The chromatically selective cells at early stages of the primate visual system do not map on to the unique hues [2,3]. Recently, however, Stoughton and Conway [4] have reported that the peak sensitivities of color cells in posterior inferior temporal cortex do cluster near the unique hues. The authors plot their results as a polar histogram: at each position on a hue circle, they show the number of cells that are maximally excited by that hue. There are three peaks in the histogram: one (the largest) falls close to unique red and another falls close to unique blue, while the third (less well-defined) lies in the yellow-green region. In fact, however, if the stimuli used in the experiment are plotted in a physiological color space, they form not a circle but an obtuse triangle. The peaks identified by Stoughton and Conway [4] fall at the apices of this triangle. Because these stimuli maximize the ratios of cone signals, they would maximally excite cells earlier in the visual system. So Stoughton and Conway's polar plot does not in itself show that cells of the posterior inferior temporal cortex represent unique hues, nor that they differ qualitatively in their behavior from chromatic cells at an earlier level

    Motion Minima for Different Directions in Color Space

    Get PDF
    AbstractWe have used the minimum-motion stimulus of Cavanagh, MacLeod and Anstis (1987) [Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 4, 1428–1438] to examine how signals along different directions in color space interact in motion perception. Stimuli were pairs of counterphasing gratings combined 90 deg out of phase in both space and time and modulated along different color-luminance axes. The axis for one of the gratings was fixed, while the axis for the second was varied so as to null perceived motion in the stimulus. The motion nulls show that observers are sensitive to motion signals carried by each of the cardinal directions of color space [an achromatic axis and L-M and S-(L + M) chromatic axes], but that signals along different cardinal axes are not combined to yield a net direction of motion. Pairing an achromatic and chromatic grating resulted in a motion null regardless of the relative or overall contrast of the two gratings, while the null directions for intermediate axes shifted depending on contrast. This result points to the special status of the luminance and chromatic axes. However, our results do not reveal a special pair of axes within the equiluminant plane. When contrasts along the cardinal axes are scaled for equal multiples of their respective detection thresholds, the L-M and S chromatic contrasts contribute roughly equally to the perceived motion, but are many times weaker than luminance contrast. Moreover, sensitivity to luminance motion is little affected by the presence of chromatic contrast, whereas sensitivity to chromatic motion is strongly masked by either luminance or chromatic contrast. These asymmetric interactions suggest that the motion of the luminance and chromatic components is encoded in qualitatively different ways. © 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd

    Multidimensional scaling reveals a color dimension unique to 'color-deficient' observers

    Get PDF
    Normal color vision depends on the relative rates at which photons are absorbed in three types of retinal cone:short-wave (S), middle-wave (M) and long-wave (L) cones, maximally sensitive near 430, 530 and 560nm, respectively. But 6% of men exhibit an X-linked variant form of color vision called deuteranomaly [1]. Their color vision is thought to depend on S cones and two forms of long-wave cone (L, L′) [2,3]. The two types of L cone contain photopigments that are maximally sensitive near 560nm, but their spectral sensitivities are different enough that the ratio of their activations gives a useful chromatic signal

    Kirschmann's Fourth Law

    Get PDF
    Kirschmann's Fourth Law states that the magnitude of simultaneous color contrast increases with the saturation of the inducing surround, but that the rate of increase reduces as saturation increases. Others since Kirschmann have agreed and disagreed. Here we show that the form of the relationship between simultaneous color contrast and inducer saturation depends on the method of measurement. Functions were measured by four methods: (i) asymmetric matching with a black surround, (ii) asymmetric matching with a surround metameric to equal energy white, (iii) dichoptic matching, and (iv) nulling an induced sinusoidal modulation. Results from the asymmetric matching conditions agreed with Kirschmann, whereas results from nulling and from dichoptic matching showed a more linear increase in simultaneous contrast with the saturation of the inducer. We conclude that the method certainly affects the conclusions reached, and that there may not be any "fair" way of measuring simultaneous contrast

    Recovered memories, satanic abuse, Dissociative Identity Disorder and false memories in the UK: a survey of Clinical Psychologists and Hypnotherapists

    Get PDF
    An online survey was conducted to examine psychological therapists’ experiences of, and beliefs about, cases of recovered memory, satanic / ritualistic abuse, Multiple Personality Disorder / Dissociative Identity Disorder, and false memory. Chartered Clinical Psychologists (n=183) and Hypnotherapists (n=119) responded. In terms of their experiences, Chartered Clinical Psychologists reported seeing more cases of satanic / ritualistic abuse compared to Hypnotherapists who, in turn, reported encountering more cases of childhood sexual abuse recovered for the first time in therapy, and more cases of suspected false memory. Chartered Clinical Psychologists were more likely to rate the essential accuracy of reports of satanic / ritualistic abuse as higher than Hypnotherapists. Belief in the accuracy of satanic / ritualistic abuse and Multiple Personality Disorder / Dissociative Identity Disorder reports correlated negatively with the belief that false memories were possible

    Is the S-opponent chromatic sub-system sluggish?

    No full text
    The S-opponent pathway has a reputation for being sluggish relative to the L/M-opponent pathway. Cottaris and De Valois [Nature 395 (1998) 896] claim that S-opponent signals are available in Macaque V1 only after 96-135 ms whereas L/M-opponent signals are available after 68-95 ms. Our experiments tested whether this large latency difference could be observed psychophysically. We measured reaction times to S/(L + M) and L/(L + M) increments. Both the equiluminant plane and the tritan line were empirically determined and we used spatio-temporal luminance noise to mask luminance cues. An adaptive staircase progressed according to observers' performance on a "go, no-go" task and provided concomitant estimates of threshold and of reaction time. When brief stimuli are confined to chromatic channels and presented at equivalent (threshold) levels and when latency is estimated from visually triggered reaction times, we find that the difference between the L/M-opponent and S-opponent sub-systems is, at most, 20-30 ms

    Signals invisible to the collicular and magnocellular pathways can capture visual attention

    No full text
    The retinal projection to the superior colliculus is thought to be important both for stimulus-driven eye movements and for the involuntary capture of attention 1, 2, 3, 4 and 5. It has further been argued that eye-movement planning and attentional orienting share common neural mechanisms 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11 and 12. Electrophysiological studies have shown that the superior colliculus receives no direct projections from short-wave-sensitive cones (S cones) 13, 14 and 15, and, consistent with this, we found that irrelevant peripheral stimuli visible only to S cones did not produce the saccadic distractor effect produced by luminance stimuli 16 and 17. However, when involuntary orienting was tested in a Posner cueing task 18 and 19, the same S-cone stimuli had normal attentional effects, in that they accelerated or delayed responses to subsequent targets. We conclude that involuntary attentional shifts do not require signals in the direct collicular pathway, or indeed the magnocellular pathway, as our S-cone stimuli were invisible to this channel also

    Suggestive association with ocular phoria at chromosome 6p22

    Get PDF
    PURPOSE We conducted a genome-wide association study to identify genetic factors that contribute to the etiology of heterophoria. METHODS We measured near and far vertical and horizontal phorias in 988 healthy adults aged 16 to 40 using the Keystone telebinocular with plates 5218 and 5219. We regressed degree of phoria against genotype at 642758 genetic loci. To control for false positives, we applied the conservative genome-wide permutation test to our data. RESULTS A locus at 6p22.2 was found to be associated with the degree of near horizontal phoria (P = 2.3 × 10(-8)). The P value resulting from a genome-wide permutation test was 0.014. CONCLUSIONS The strongest association signal arose from an intronic region of the gene ALDH5A1, which encodes the mitochondrial enzyme succinic semialdehyde dehydrogenase (SSADH), an enzyme involved in γ-aminobutyric acid metabolism. Succinic semialdehyde dehydrogenase deficiency, resulting from mutations of ALDH5A1, causes a variety of neural and behavioral abnormalities, including strabismus. Variation in ALDH5A1 is likely to contribute to degree of horizontal phoria
    corecore