29 research outputs found

    Evaluation of legibility of not properly reflecting signs

    Get PDF
    in conditions of dew or temperatures below 0 degrees C the reflection of reflecting material on signs can be suboptimal. Problems with respect to legibility under such conditions were studied, as well as subjective ratings of the signed information. Images of the signs were presented using an LCD projector. Independent variables in the experiment were age, amount of information, stimulus quality, and presentation time. Dependent variables were errors in reproduction of the names of the cities, towns or villages on the signs, and subjective ratings. It turned out that all factors both separately and additively affected the amount of errors. Positive subjective ratings coincided mainly with level of reflection, and to a lesser degree with the amount of information, presentation time and familiarity of information. Relevance to industry: Different types of reflecting material have been developed to reflect light from signs optimally. Disadvantage of some of the materials is the effect dew and temperatures below 0 degrees C can have on reflection and legibility. The study has practical consequences in terms of novelty and amount of information to be presented on such a sign. (c) 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved

    Inexperience and risky decisions of young adolescents, as pedestrians and cyclists, in interactions with lorries, and the effects of competency versus awareness education

    Get PDF
    Background Road injuries are a prime cause of death in early adolescence. Often road safety education (RSE) is used to target risky road behaviour in this age group. These RSE programmes are frequently based on the assumption that deliberate risk taking rather than lack of competency underlies risk behaviour. This study tested the competency of 10-13 year olds, by examining their decisions - as pedestrians and cyclists - in dealing with blind spot areas around lorries. Also, the effects of an awareness programme and a competency programme on these decisions were evaluated. Method Table-top models were used, representing seven scenarios that differed in complexity: one basic scenario to test the identification of blind spot areas, and 6 traffic scenarios to test behaviour in traffic situations of low or high task complexity. Using a quasi-experimental design (pre-test and post-test reference group design without randomization), the programme effects were assessed by requiring participants (n = 62) to show, for each table-top traffic scenario, how they would act if they were in that traffic situation. Results On the basic scenario, at pre-test 42% of the youngsters identified all blind spots correctly, but only 27% showed safe behaviour in simple scenarios and 5% in complex scenarios. The competency programme yielded improved performance on the basic scenario but not on the traffic scenarios, whereas the awareness programme did not result in any improvements. The correlation between improvements on the basic scenarios and the traffic scenarios was not significant. Conclusions Young adolescents have not yet mastered the necessary skills for safe performance in simple and complex traffic situations, thus underlining the need for effective prevention programmes. RSE may improve the understanding of blind spot areas but this does not 'automatically' transfer to performance in traffic situations. Implications for the design of RSE are discussed. © 2013 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved

    PREVENT: education to improve road safety around work zones

    No full text
    The relatively high traffic accident rate around work zones is partly caused by driver error. To reduce driver error and to improve driver behaviour around workzones, a training programme was developed within the framework of the European Union "Leonardo Da Vinci" "PREVENT" project. Literature reviews and accident statistics analyses were carried out to define the objectives of the training programme with respect to crash circumstances, driver errors and violations. This paper presents the outcome of these analyses. The paper demonstrates how these findings were used to define the content of the educational tools necessary for improving driver behaviour around work zones. Based on human factors literature, the paper also discusses the possibilities and limitations of education in improving work zone safety and driver performance. It shows that adequate accident and driver data are essential for developing education programmes

    Effects of emotions on optimism bias and illusion of control in traffic.

    No full text
    Anger is related to aggression in traffic, which in turn may lead to risky driving (e.g. Lajunen, Parker & Stradling, 1998; Deffenbacher, Huff, Lynch, Oetting & Salvatore, 2000; Parker, Lajunen & Summa!a, 2002; Deffenbacher, Lynch, Filetti, Dahlen & Oetting, 2003). Anger may also lead directly to risk taking behaviour. Although evidence for the link between anger and risk exists in areas outside traffic (Lerner & Keltner, 2001; Lerner, Gonalez, Small & Fischoff, 2003), the research that has been carried out in the area of traffic is mostly correlational and therefore leaves space for alternative explanations. For example, Arnett, Offer and Fine (1996) showed that people who are in an angry state exceed the speed limit to a greater degree than people who are not angry. This might imply that an angry state leads to speeding behaviour. An alternative explanation is that certain types of people are more inclined to both experience anger and to exceed the speed limit. For example, Sensation Seeking (defined by Zuckerman (1994) as "the need to seek novel, varied, complex and intense sensations and experiences") has been associated with both anger (Zuckerman, 1994; Iversen & Rundmo, 2002) and risk (Jonah, 1997; Jonah, Thiessen & Au-Yeung, 2001; Iversen & Rundmo, 2002; Roberti, 2004). In the present study, the effects of anger on cognitive processes related to risk will be investigated. Two types of emotion effects on performance can be distinguished. Emotion may consciously trigger actions or action tendencies (Frijda, 1986), as is the case in aggression, or emotions may cause a bias in cognition (Clore & Gasper, 2000). A number of cognitive processes may be affected by emotions, for instance memory (Parrot & Spackman, 2000) and social judgement (Forgas, 1995, 1998). However, not all of these processes are related to risk taking behaviour and as such relevant for traffic safety. Two cognitive processes that are related to risk can be distinguished: optimism bias and illusion of control. Optimism (or optimistic) bias refers to the extent to which people are biased about their chances of getting involved in good or bad events. The term is also referred to as unrealistic optimism and was first described by (Weinstein, 1980). In two studies, Weinstein showed that people rate their chances to experience positive events as higher than average and their chances to experience negative events as lower than average. It is important to keep in mind that optimism bias can only be determined at group level. An individual person may rate his or her risk as lower than average and may actually be correct. However, on a group level, it is unlikely that the risk of the majority is below average. Also in the area of traffic, research shows that people tend to overestimate their driving skills and underestimate their risk of getting involved in a car crash. Several studies (Svensson, 1981; Svensson, Fischhoff & MacGregor, 1985; Dejoy, 1989;) showed that drivers are optimistically biased: they rate themselves as more skillful and safer than the average driver, and they rate their accident likelihood as less than the average driver. Perceived controllability of the accident is a strong predictor of optimism in these studies. Also McKenna (1993) found that the optimism bias disappears when taking perceived control into account. So, people think they are better drivers only when they think they have control over the situation. In general, people tend to overestimate the degree of control they can exert on a situation. Langer (1975) called this phenomenon the illusion of control. In her studies, the concept of illusion of control refers to situations in which the control is really absent. However, in traffic related studies, the concept is mostly used to refer to situations where the control is genuine, but the benefits of the control are illusory. Horswill and McKenna (1999a,b) found differences in preferred speed when people were asked to imagine they were driving themselves, and when people were asked to imagine they were a passenger. As drivers, people accepted higher speeds than as passengers, presumably because as passengers they thought their hypothetical driver was less able to cope with higher speed than themselves. The control as a driver is genuine, but this control does not necessarily lead to a decreased risk level. Several studies have shown relations between emotions and these cognitive processes. Dewberry and Richardson (1990) and Dewberry, Ing, James, Nixon and Richardson (1990) showed an inverse relationship between anxiety and optimism bias: people who were more anxious about negative life events were less inclined to be unrealistically optimistic. The authors conclude that anxiety reduces optimism, and as anxiety is an example of negative affect, the authors furthermore generalise that not only anxiety, but also negative affect in general, reduces optimism. Several studies have examined the effect of specific emotions, as opposed to general positive or negative affect, on cognitive processes. Some older studies investigated the relationship between emotions and illusion of control. Alloy and Abramson (1979) and Alloy, Abramson and Viscusi (1982) found that depressed persons are less vulnerable to the illusion of control than non-depressed persons. Lemer and Keltner (2001) found opposing effects offear and anger on risk perception: angry respondents rated situations as less risky than fearful respondents. Hemenover and Zhang (2004) showed that anger is related to optimistic evaluations, which is contrary to the conclusion of Dewberry that general negative affect reduces optimism. The last two studies used the appraisal tendency framework (Lemer & Keltner, 2000) to explain emotion-specific effects on cognitive processes. According to this framework, people who are in a specific emotional state are likely to interpret other (not necessarily related) events in line with the emotions. For example, as anger is associated with a high level of perceived control, angry people will rate situations as more controllable and therefore less risky than non-angry individuals. Sad and fearful respondents will rate situations as less controllable and therefore as more risky, whereas happy persons will display the same pattern as angry respondents. The general aim of the study reported here was to investigate the effect of anger on cognitive bias in traffic. An experimental design was used in which subjects either did or did not receive an emotion induction procedure. All subjects made judgements of traffic situations and the performance of the two groups was compared. The emotions that people experience and express in certain situations are different from person to person. As mentioned before, sensation seeking has been shown to be related to the experience of anger (Zuckerman, 1994). Therefore, in the current study the sensation seeking scale is included. It is a personality scale that aims to measure the tendency of people to engage in exciting or thrilling activities. Within the sensation seeking scale, four subscales can be distinguished: Thrill and adventure seeking, experience seeking, boredom susceptibility and disinhibition. It is expected that the emotion manipulation procedure works best with people who score high on sensation seeking. Another reason to include sensation seeking in the experiment, is that relations have been shown between sensation seeking and risk taking behaviour (Horvath & Zuckerman, 1993; Heino, 1996; Jonah et al., 2001)

    Interpersonal violations, speeding violations and their relation to accident involvement in Finland

    No full text
    The aim of the present study was to replicate the distinction between errors, lapses and violations, and to identify aggressive violations from normal or highway code violations. Furthermore, the relationship of these behaviours with road traffic accidents was examined. A total number of 1126 Finnish drivers completed a questionnaire containing the Driver Behaviour Questionnaire (DBQ) with extended violations scale, and questions regarding background information, such as age, gender and mileage. Also, questions about previous accidents and fines were asked. Factor analysis showed that a four-factor structure seemed more appropriate than the earlier established three-factor structure. The four factors were errors, lapses, speeding violations and interpersonal violations. The two types of violations result from different motives, and seem to be associated with different kinds of affect. Both interpersonal and speeding violations were reported most by young males, which was consistent with earlier findings. Logistic regression analyses indicated that errors predicted active accident involvement after partialling out the effects of demographic variables, whereas interpersonal violations were positively related to involvement in passive accidents. This was presumably due to different reporting tendencies of respondents. Speeding tickets were predicted by speeding and interpersonal violations and lapses and penalties for speeding by both kinds of violations and errors. Penalties for speeding, parking and other offences were predicted by interpersonal violations. The implications of these results are discussed
    corecore