16 research outputs found

    Differences in gaze anticipation for locomotion with and without vision

    Get PDF
    International audiencePrevious experimental studies have shown a spontaneous anticipation of locomotor trajectory by the head and gaze direction during human locomotion. This anticipatory behavior could serve several functions: an optimal selection of visual information, for instance through landmarks and optic flow, as well as trajectory planning and motor control. This would imply that anticipation remains in darkness but with different characteristics. We asked 10 participants to walk along two predefined complex trajectories (limaçon and figure eight) without any cue on the trajectory to follow. Two visual conditions were used: (i) in light and (ii) in complete darkness with eyes open. The whole body kinematics were recorded by motion capture, along with the participant's right eye movements. We showed that in darkness and in light, horizontal gaze anticipates the orientation of the head which itself anticipates the trajectory direction. However, the horizontal angular anticipation decreases by a half in darkness for both gaze and head. In both visual conditions we observed an eye nystagmus with similar properties (frequency and amplitude). The main difference comes from the fact that in light, there is a shift of the orientations of the eye nystagmus and the head in the direction of the trajectory. These results suggest that a fundamental function of gaze is to represent self motion, stabilize the perception of space during locomotion, and to simulate the future trajectory, regardless of the vision condition

    Adaptive Gaze Strategies for Locomotion with Constricted Visual Field

    Get PDF
    In retinitis pigmentosa (RP), loss of peripheral visual field accounts for most difficulties encountered in visuo-motor coordination during locomotion. The purpose of this study was to accurately assess the impact of peripheral visual field loss on gaze strategies during locomotion, and identify compensatory mechanisms. Nine RP subjects presenting a central visual field limited to 10–25° in diameter, and nine healthy subjects were asked to walk in one of three directions—straight ahead to a visual target, leftward and rightward through a door frame, with or without obstacle on the way. Whole body kinematics were recorded by motion capture, and gaze direction in space was reconstructed using an eye-tracker. Changes in gaze strategies were identified in RP subjects, including extensive exploration prior to walking, frequent fixations of the ground (even knowing no obstacle was present), of door edges, essentially of the proximal one, of obstacle edge/corner, and alternating door edges fixations when approaching the door. This was associated with more frequent, sometimes larger rapid-eye-movements, larger movements, and forward tilting of the head. Despite the visual handicap, the trajectory geometry was identical between groups, with a small decrease in walking speed in RPs. These findings identify the adaptive changes in sensory-motor coordination, in order to ensure visual awareness of the surrounding, detect changes in spatial configuration, collect information for self-motion, update the postural reference frame, and update egocentric distances to environmental objects. They are of crucial importance for the design of optimized rehabilitation procedures

    Mean experimental characteristics of optical flows as a function of gaze orientation.

    No full text
    <p>The optical flow speeds at the fixation position for the two stimuli are presented here; for the largest radius of curvature trajectories (white histogram); and for the sharpest one (black histogram). The absolute angular deviation between the two optical flow vectors is also represented (dashed line). Bars indicate between-subjects standard error.</p

    Examples of trajectories and camera orientations used in the experiment.

    No full text
    <p><b>A.</b> The observers' task was to judge the relative curvature between two simulated trajectories of constant radii R1 (green trajectory) and R2 (red trajectory). These radii were centered around a target trajectory, fixed to 200 m of radius, and separated by an adjustable step quantity ( difference at the beginning, i.e. 70.58 m giving successive trajectories of 235.29 m and 164.70 m radii), following a PEST procedure. <b>B.</b> In different, experimental conditions, the camera could be rotated at five constant directions defined by multiples of the eccentricity of the tangent point direction . This quantity was computed from the tangent point location for a curve of 200 m radius of curvature and an ‘imaginary’ 3.5 m wide road. <b>C.</b> Representation of the flow fields corresponding to the five camera orientations for a single trajectory of 200 m of radius of curvature. A counter rotation, function of the simulated camera rotation, was applied to the virtual environment, such that the observers' actual gaze was always positioned in the center of the display screen.</p

    Mean percentages of path curvature discrimination thresholds, as a function of gaze orientation.

    No full text
    <p>The threshold is the percentage of difference between radii of curvature that observers were able to discriminate with of correct responses. The direction corresponds to the minimal optical flow velocity and to the best discrimination performance. Bars indicate between-subjects standard error.</p

    Two alternative forced choice protocol (2AFC).

    No full text
    <p><b>A.</b> Schematic temporal arrangement of half of one trial. Subjects were first required to fixate a red cross on a black background. After 500 ms, random dots appeared, remained static for 500 ms and then move for 500 ms. A black screen then appeared for 500 ms, followed by the second stimulus. <b>B.</b> Comparison stimuli for a single 2AFC trial. In this example, the first stimulus displays a 50 m radius of curvature trajectory and the second a 350 m one. Observers' task was to judge which one was curved the most. Please note that colors are inverted for printing purposes.</p

    An optical velocity field generated by a circular trajectory parallel to the ground plane and aligned with road geometry.

    No full text
    <p>The edge-lines of the road are represented by continuous black lines and the tangent point by a red dot. The virtual line (in red) corresponds to an inversion of the horizontal component of optic flow velocity. The tangent point is the intersection between the red line and the edge-line.</p

    Comparison between different areas of optical flow integration in the model.

    No full text
    <p>The model was fitted to the data (for the five values of gaze orientation) for the averaged experimental thresholds by seeking the best that minimized the root mean square error (RMSE) of the model over the data. Different sizes of optical flow integration areas were tested, from a single foveal point to a disc with a diameter of 20 degrees, centered on the gaze position. The best values, the RMSE of the model over the data, the Pearson R and its square are presented for each integration size. The bold line indicates the best integration area (smallest RMSE), achieved for a 5 degree area.</p

    Comparison between model predictions for different areas of optical flow integration.

    No full text
    <p>The path curvature discrimination thresholds are represented as a function of gaze orientation. Average data from all subjects are shown in black with the bars indicating between-subjects standard error. The model predictions are represented by colored solid lines, from 0 degrees of integration (i.e. punctual optical flow) to 5 and 10 degree circular areas.</p

    Adaptive Gaze Strategies for Locomotion with Constricted Visual Field

    Get PDF
    In retinitis pigmentosa (RP), loss of peripheral visual field accounts for most difficulties encountered in visuo-motor coordination during locomotion. The purpose of this study was to accurately assess the impact of peripheral visual field loss on gaze strategies during locomotion, and identify compensatory mechanisms. Nine RP subjects presenting a central visual field limited to 10–25° in diameter, and nine healthy subjects were asked to walk in one of three directions—straight ahead to a visual target, leftward and rightward through a door frame, with or without obstacle on the way. Whole body kinematics were recorded by motion capture, and gaze direction in space was reconstructed using an eye-tracker. Changes in gaze strategies were identified in RP subjects, including extensive exploration prior to walking, frequent fixations of the ground (even knowing no obstacle was present), of door edges, essentially of the proximal one, of obstacle edge/corner, and alternating door edges fixations when approaching the door. This was associated with more frequent, sometimes larger rapid-eye-movements, larger movements, and forward tilting of the head. Despite the visual handicap, the trajectory geometry was identical between groups, with a small decrease in walking speed in RPs. These findings identify the adaptive changes in sensory-motor coordination, in order to ensure visual awareness of the surrounding, detect changes in spatial configuration, collect information for self-motion, update the postural reference frame, and update egocentric distances to environmental objects. They are of crucial importance for the design of optimized rehabilitation procedures
    corecore