14 research outputs found

    Definition of Loss Aversion (LA) and Correlation Between Measures.

    No full text
    <p><b>(a)</b> The local definition of LA focuses on the slopes of the value function on the either side of an inflection point between approach and avoidance (s+ and s- respectively), or gains and losses. Measures of s+ and s- are collected close to the origin (see green and purple boxes), where the scale of value will minimally bias assessments of risk. The slopes <b>(b)</b> s+ and <b>(c)</b> s- are schematized for two representative curves from one individual. <b>(d)</b> LA is computed by the absolute value of the ratio of s- to s+, and is summed over the 10% of the graph on either side of the origin or inflection point. LA from this graph is quite similar to that reported by Kahneman and Tversky [<a href="http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0135216#pone.0135216.ref005" target="_blank">5</a>,<a href="http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0135216#pone.0135216.ref042" target="_blank">42</a>]. <b>(e)</b> Correlation of LA from the anticipatory phase of the PT-based task and from the RPT-based task, showing a significant effect after correction for multiple comparisons.</p

    The Commonality of Loss Aversion across Procedures and Stimuli

    Get PDF
    <div><p>Individuals tend to give losses approximately 2-fold the weight that they give gains. Such approximations of loss aversion (LA) are almost always measured in the stimulus domain of money, rather than objects or pictures. Recent work on preference-based decision-making with a schedule-less keypress task (relative preference theory, RPT) has provided a mathematical formulation for LA similar to that in prospect theory (PT), but makes no parametric assumptions in the computation of LA, uses a variable tied to communication theory (i.e., the Shannon entropy or information), and works readily with non-monetary stimuli. We evaluated if these distinct frameworks described similar LA in healthy subjects, and found that LA during the anticipation phase of the PT-based task correlated significantly with LA related to the RPT-based task. Given the ease with which non-monetary stimuli can be used on the Internet, or in animal studies, these findings open an extensive range of applications for the study of loss aversion. Furthermore, the emergence of methodology that can be used to measure preference for both social stimuli and money brings a common framework to the evaluation of preference in both social psychology and behavioral economics.</p></div

    Experimental Procedures and Resulting Value Functions.

    No full text
    <p><b>(a)</b> The PT-based experiment used two “gambles”, schematized by two spinners. One spinner showed two-thirds of its area as gains (+10)andone−thirdaslosses(−10) and one-third as losses (-8), leading to an expected outcome (i.e., referred to as actuarial outcome in Breiter et al. [<a href="http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0135216#pone.0135216.ref038" target="_blank">38</a>]) of +4.Thesecondspinnershowedone−thirdsofitsareaasgains(+4. The second spinner showed one-thirds of its area as gains (+10) and two-thirds as losses (-8),leadingtoanexpectedoutcomeof−8), leading to an expected outcome of-2. Each trial lasted 20 seconds, with 10s focused on the arrow spinning (anticipation phase) and 10s focused on the arrow stopping, and the win/loss flickering (outcome phase). Order of presentation between the PT-based experiment and RPT-based experiment was counterbalanced across subjects. <b>(b)</b> The RPT-based experiment used a keypress procedure [<a href="http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0135216#pone.0135216.ref021" target="_blank">21</a>]: a picture would appear for 200ms, then be replaced by a fixation point for 1800ms. After 2000ms, the face would reappear, and if subjects did nothing, the face would stay up another 6000ms (e.g., default condition). Subjects could increase viewing time via a scalloping resistive function, getting close to maximum 1400ms. Alternatively, they could decrease viewing time with the same resistive function close to a minimum of 2000ms. The scalloping resistive function incrementally reduced the viewing time alteration achieved by each keypress, so subjects needed to exert effort to increase or reduce viewing times. Its mathematical formulation can be found in Kim et al. [<a href="http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0135216#pone.0135216.ref021" target="_blank">21</a>], along with multiple control analyses about its impact on subject behavior. <b>(c)</b> The value function for the PT-based experiment mapped subjective ratings made during the anticipation phase of the experiment on the y-axis, and the actuarial amount of the spinner on the x-axis. For the outcome phase of this experiment, the value function mapped the subjective ratings made when the arrow stopped spinning against the gain or loss. <b>(d)</b> The RPT-based graph showed the mean intensity of keypressing to increase viewtime (K<sub>+</sub>) or decrease viewtime (K<sub>-</sub>) calibrated against the Shannon entropy of keypress patterns to increase (H<sub>+</sub>) or decrease (H<sub>-</sub>) viewtime. Solid and empty triangles stand for individual data points for the five categories of facial expressions.</p

    Voxel based morphometry in cervical dystonia.

    No full text
    <p>Voxel based morphometry demonstrated reduced gray matter local tissue volume in the posterior cingulate (A and B, blue voxels, shown at two significance thresholds, presented as family-wise uncorrected p-values), but no differences in the thalamus, in cervical dystonia (family-wise error corrected p = 0.9996). When the analysis was restricted to only those voxels in a thalamic mask (to minimize the loss of statistical power by multiple-comparisons correction; C, green voxels), no significant differences in local tissue volume were noted (p = 0.34). Significant voxels (A, B) and thalamic mask (C) overlie the mean gray matter structural image. Note that identical structural scans were used in VBM analyses and segmentation analyses (i.e., scans used in this figure were the same as those used for data in Figs <a href="http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0155302#pone.0155302.g002" target="_blank">2</a>, <a href="http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0155302#pone.0155302.g003" target="_blank">3</a> and <a href="http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0155302#pone.0155302.g005" target="_blank">5</a>). VBM results were corrected using threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE). All axial and coronal views are from a single plane, indicated in MNI Talairach coordinates. Color bars at bottom indicate TFCE-corrected p-values for the images above. Abbreviations: pat = patients; ctrl = controls. R = Right hemisphere; L = Left hemisphere.</p

    Demographic information and gross volumetric negative-control contrasts.

    No full text
    <p>Demographic and volumetric negative-control measures for each experimental group demonstrate high group similarity. Within each experimental group (cervical dystonia–CD; spasmodic dysphonia–SD), controls were matched to patients for gender, handedness, and age +/- five years (A). Each volumetric negative-control measure is expressed as the percentage of mean control volume (<i>e</i>.<i>g</i>., estimated total intracranial volume (eTIV) for CD patients is 2% larger for patients than for matched controls). No large-scale volumetric measures differed between patients and controls (A, p-values uncorrected). The reduction of thalamic volume in patients maintained significance following normalization for all volumetric negative-control measures (B).</p

    Regional automated and manual gross volume measures.

    No full text
    <p>A reduction in thalamic volume, not seen in other regions involved in the control of movement, was seen in both cervical dystonia and spasmodic dysphonia. Total volume (i.e., number of voxels in left plus right hemispheres) is shown for each region of interest (mean ± standard error of the mean). Given the large differences in volume between brain regions, the axis has been adjusted to focus on each cluster of values. Breaks in the y-axis are indicated by hashed horizontal bars. All p-values corrected for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni corrected significance threshold, p = 0.00625); * p≀0.0060; ** p = 0.00020. Abbreviation: BA6 = Brodmann Area 6; Thal auto = automated thalamic segmentation.</p

    Relative regional gross volume measures in focal dystonia.

    No full text
    <p>Relative gross thalamic volume, normalized to matched controls, was less in dystonia patients. Total gross thalamic volume was normalized to the mean value for matched controls, expressed as percent difference ± standard error of the mean for each region of interest. This data is reformatted but otherwise identical to that in <a href="http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0155302#pone.0155302.g002" target="_blank">Fig 2</a>. All p-values corrected for multiple comparisons, (Bonferroni corrected significance threshold, p = 0.00625); * p≀0.0060; ** p = 0.00020. Abbreviations: CD = cervical dystonia; SD = spasmodic dysphonia; BA6 = Brodmann Area 6.</p

    Relationship of clinical measures to gross thalamic volume in CD patients.

    No full text
    <p>The relationship between individual gross thalamic volume (manually segmented, in mm<sup>3</sup>) and clinical measures suggests that reduced gross thalamic volume is a risk factor for dystonia, and is not a secondary effect of dystonia symptoms. Data are shown for all variables, even if excluded from regression models, so raw data for CD and SD can be viewed and compared. Both CD patients and controls (A) showed declining volume with age. Patient:control gross thalamic volume showed a qualitative divergence with age between CD and controls, but the divergence was not statistically significant. (B) There was no relationship between volume and age in the SD cohort (for either SD or controls), presumably reflecting the smaller age range in this cohort. Likewise, there was no divergence of slopes with age between SD and controls. Age at CD onset (C) appeared to correlate with gross thalamic volume, but this was likely driven by the relationship between age at scan and age of onset; age at SD onset (D) was not correlated with volume. Gross thalamic volume did not correlate significantly with duration for either CD (E); this relationship was not evaluated statistically for SD due to collinearity with other variables, but the positive slope suggests no indication of a decline in volume with increasing duration (F). Thalamic volume also did not correlate with severity of dystonia for either CD or SD in the multiple regression model, as measured by the Tsui scale for CD (G) or the voice-related quality of life score for SD (H), although the SD relationship to severity showed a trend toward significance (V-RQOL, p = 0.056), and appeared significant when evaluated post hoc as a single variable (p = 0.012). The asymmetry of muscles affected with cervical dystonia (as gauged by laterality of units of botulinum toxin injected) did not correlate with asymmetries in thalamic volume (I, p = 0.89). Note that for (C) and (D), thalamic volumes for control subjects are plotted vs. the age at onset for the matched patient, as control subjects do not have an age at onset. Volume for control subjects is included in C and D as a reference only (designated by Ω), to illustrate that patient:control differences persist (and in fact are more robust) when demographics (including age) are matched: with the exception of a single CD/control dyad, every patient showed lower volume than his/her matched control.</p

    Clinical characteristics of cervical dystonia patients.

    No full text
    *<p>subjects included in 2006 study using different analyses.</p><p>BFM: Burke Fahn Marsden dystonia rating scale.</p><p>Tsui: Tsui rating scale for cervical dystonia.</p><p>TW: Toronto Western Spasmodic Torticollis Rating Scale (TWSTRS) for cervical dyston.</p

    Regions of probabilistic diffusion tractography that were significantly different between cervical dystonia patients and controls.

    No full text
    <p>(A) shows reduced tractography from the left AL seed region, and (B) shows elevated tractography from the right pallidal seed region. Arrows point to the region through which we drew ROIs in our previous DTI study, which includes AL fibers. <i>A priori</i> segmented regions are shown as reference points: Pink = red nucleus; White = substantia nigra; Blue = pedunculopontine nucleus. MNI talairach coordinates are indicated for each two dimensional image. Lower images in each panel show three dimensional rendering of clusters; the image for the left AL includes the AL seed region in green, and the patient/control difference is shown in blue. t maps and three dimensional clusters are superimposed on the average FA map for all 24 subjects in the study for anatomical reference, and are thresholded at t = +/−2.07 (the threshold used to identify difference clusters, p<0.05, uncorrected, for df = 22). The color bars indicate the range of t values in each panel, from +/−2.07 to the peak t value for each contrast. Warm tones (red, orange, yellow) indicate regions in which cervical dystonia patients exhibited elevated tractography relative to control subjects. Cool tones (blues) indicate regions in which cervical dystonia patients exhibited reduced tractography relative to control subjects. Three dimensional images are shown in mono-color rather than graded/multi-color to illustrate location rather than significance. LH: left hemisphere; RH: right hemisphere.</p
    corecore