230 research outputs found
Sensitive periods, but not critical periods, evolve in a fluctuating environment: a model of incremental development
Item does not contain fulltextSensitive periods, during which the impact of experience on phenotype is larger than in other periods, exist in all classes of organisms, yet little is known about their evolution. Recent mathematical modelling has explored the conditions in which natural selection favours sensitive periods. These models have assumed that the environment is stable across ontogeny or that organisms can develop phenotypes instantaneously at any age. Neither assumption generally holds. Here, we present a model in which organisms gradually tailor their phenotypes to an environment that fluctuates across ontogeny, while receiving cost-free, imperfect cues to the current environmental state. We vary the rate of environmental change, the reliability of cues and the duration of adulthood relative to ontogeny. We use stochastic dynamic programming to compute optimal policies. From these policies, we simulate levels of plasticity across ontogeny and obtain mature phenotypes. Our results show that sensitive periods can occur at the onset, midway through and even towards the end of ontogeny. In contrast with models assuming stable environments, organisms always retain residual plasticity late in ontogeny. We conclude that critical periods, after which plasticity is zero, are unlikely to be favoured in environments that fluctuate across ontogeny.10 p
An evolutionary model of sensitive periods when the reliability of cues varies across ontogeny
Sensitive periods are widespread in nature, but their evolution is not well understood. Recent mathematical modeling has illuminated the conditions favoring the evolution of sensitive periods early in ontogeny. However, sensitive periods also exist at later stages of ontogeny, such as adolescence. Here, we present a mathematical model that explores the conditions that favor sensitive periods at later developmental stages. In our model, organisms use environmental cues to incrementally construct a phenotype that matches their environment. Unlike in previous models, the reliability of cues varies across ontogeny. We use stochastic dynamic programming to compute optimal policies for a range of evolutionary ecologies and then simulate developmental trajectories to obtain mature phenotypes. We measure changes in plasticity across ontogeny using study paradigms inspired by empirical research: adoption and cross-fostering. Our results show that sensitive periods only evolve later in ontogeny if the reliability of cues increases across ontogeny. The onset, duration, and offset of sensitive periods—and the magnitude of plasticity—depend on the specific parameter settings. If the reliability of cues decreases across ontogeny, sensitive periods are favored only early in ontogeny. These results are robust across different paradigms suggesting that empirical findings might be comparable despite different experimental designs
Sensitive periods, but not critical periods, evolve in a fluctuating environment:: a model of incremental development
Sensitive periods, during which the impact of experience on phenotype is larger than in other periods, exist in all classes of organisms, yet little is known about their evolution. Recent mathematical modelling has explored the conditions in which natural selection favours sensitive periods. These models have assumed that the environment is stable across ontogeny or that organisms can develop phenotypes instantaneously at any age. Neither assumption generally holds. Here, we present a model in which organisms gradually tailor their phenotypes to an environment that fluctuates across ontogeny, while receiving cost-free, imperfect cues to the current environmental state. We vary the rate of environmental change, the reliability of cues and the duration of adulthood relative to ontogeny. We use stochastic dynamic programming to compute optimal policies. From these policies, we simulate levels of plasticity across ontogeny and obtain mature phenotypes. Our results show that sensitive periods can occur at the onset, midway through and even towards the end of ontogeny. In contrast with models assuming stable environments, organisms always retain residual plasticity late in ontogeny. We conclude that critical periods, after which plasticity is zero, are unlikely to be favoured in environments that fluctuate across ontogeny
Biology, Society, or Choice: How Do Non-Experts Interpret Explanations of Behaviour?
Explanations for human behaviour can be framed in many different ways, from the social-structural context to the individual motivation down to the neurobiological implementation. We know comparatively little about how people interpret these explanatory framings, and what they infer when one kind of explanation rather than another is made salient. In four experiments, UK general-population volunteers read vignettes describing the same behaviour, but providing explanations framed in different ways. In Study 1, we found that participants grouped explanations into ‘biological’, ‘psychological’ and ‘sociocultural’ clusters. Explanations with different framings were often seen as incompatible with one another, especially when one belonged to the ‘biological’ cluster and the other did not. In Study 2, we found that exposure to a particular explanatory framing triggered inferences beyond the information given. Specifically, psychological explanations led participants to assume the behaviour was malleable, and biological framings led them to assume it was not. In Studies 3A and 3B, we found that the choice of explanatory framing can affect people’s assumptions about effective interventions. For example, presenting a biological explanation increased people’s conviction that interventions like drugs would be effective, and decreased their conviction that psychological or socio-political interventions would be effective. These results illuminate the intuitive psychology of explanations, and also potential pitfalls in scientific communication. Framing an explanation in a particular way will often generate inferences in the audience—about what other factors are not causally important, how easy it is to change the behaviour, and what kinds of remedies are worth considering—that the communicator may not have anticipated and might not intend
Visible light guided manipulation of liquid wettability on photoresponsive surfaces
Photoresponsive titania surfaces are of great interest due to their unique wettability change upon ultraviolet light illumination. However, their applications are often limited either by the inability to respond to visible light or the need for special treatment to recover the original wettability. Sensitizing TiO2 surfaces with visible light-absorbing materials has been utilized in photovoltaic applications. Here we demonstrate that a dye-sensitized TiO2 surface can selectively change the wettability towards contacting liquids upon visible light illumination due to a photo-induced voltage across the liquid and the underlying surface. The photo-induced wettability change of our surfaces enables external manipulation of liquid droplet motion upon illumination. We show demulsification of surfactant-stabilized brine-in-oil emulsions via coalescence of brine droplets on our dye-sensitized TiO2 surface upon visible light illumination. We anticipate that our surfaces will have a wide range of applications including microfluidic devices with customizable wettability, solar-driven oil–water clean-up and demulsification technologies
Adaptive gene regulatory networks
Regulatory interactions between genes show a large amount of cross-species
variability, even when the underlying functions are conserved: There are many
ways to achieve the same function. Here we investigate the ability of
regulatory networks to reproduce given expression levels within a simple model
of gene regulation. We find an exponentially large space of regulatory networks
compatible with a given set of expression levels, giving rise to an extensive
entropy of networks. Typical realisations of regulatory networks are found to
share a bias towards symmetric interactions, in line with empirical evidence.Comment: 5 pages RevTe
Recommended from our members
Microstructural characterization of high energy product Nd-Fe-B rapidly solidified ribbons
The bonded Nd-Fe-B market has experienced the fastest growth of any permanent magnet market. Rapidly solidified Nd-Fe-B forms the basis for this bonded magnet industry. Rapid solidification is carried out by melt spinning, producing a highly stable and magnetically hard microstructure. This study focuses on a microstructural analysis of this melt spun ribbon using transmission and scanning electron microscopy (TEM and SEM), atom probe field ion microscopy (APFIM) and Auger electron spectroscopy (AES). The ribbons showed a uniform grain size and shape distribution through the thickness of the melt spun ribbon. Grain sizes ranged from 20 to 40 nm. AES showed neodymium enrichment on a fracture surface and corresponding iron and boron depletion suggesting that the eutectic 70Nd-30Fe phase is present with a thickness of approximately 1 nm. Atom probe composition analysis of grain interiors gave results very close to the nominal composition and some preliminary evidence of a grain boundary phase
If cooperation is likely punish mildly: Insights from economic experiments based on the snowdrift game
Punishment may deter antisocial behavior. Yet to punish is costly, and the
costs often do not offset the gains that are due to elevated levels of
cooperation. However, the effectiveness of punishment depends not only on how
costly it is, but also on the circumstances defining the social dilemma. Using
the snowdrift game as the basis, we have conducted a series of economic
experiments to determine whether severe punishment is more effective than mild
punishment. We have observed that severe punishment is not necessarily more
effective, even if the cost of punishment is identical in both cases. The
benefits of severe punishment become evident only under extremely adverse
conditions, when to cooperate is highly improbable in the absence of sanctions.
If cooperation is likely, mild punishment is not less effective and leads to
higher average payoffs, and is thus the much preferred alternative. Presented
results suggest that the positive effects of punishment stem not only from
imposed fines, but may also have a psychological background. Small fines can do
wonders in motivating us to chose cooperation over defection, but without the
paralyzing effect that may be brought about by large fines. The later should be
utilized only when absolutely necessary.Comment: 15 pages, 6 figures; accepted for publication in PLoS ON
Sustainable institutionalized punishment requires elimination of second-order free-riders
Although empirical and theoretical studies affirm that punishment can elevate
collaborative efforts, its emergence and stability remain elusive. By
peer-punishment the sanctioning is something an individual elects to do
depending on the strategies in its neighborhood. The consequences of
unsustainable efforts are therefore local. By pool-punishment, on the other
hand, where resources for sanctioning are committed in advance and at large,
the notion of sustainability has greater significance. In a population with
free-riders, punishers must be strong in numbers to keep the "punishment pool"
from emptying. Failure to do so renders the concept of institutionalized
sanctioning futile. We show that pool-punishment in structured populations is
sustainable, but only if second-order free-riders are sanctioned as well, and
to a such degree that they cannot prevail. A discontinuous phase transition
leads to an outbreak of sustainability when punishers subvert second-order
free-riders in the competition against defectors.Comment: 7 two-column pages, 3 figures; accepted for publication in Scientific
Report
If players are sparse social dilemmas are too: Importance of percolation for evolution of cooperation
Spatial reciprocity is a well known tour de force of cooperation promotion. A
thorough understanding of the effects of different population densities is
therefore crucial. Here we study the evolution of cooperation in social
dilemmas on different interaction graphs with a certain fraction of vacant
nodes. We find that sparsity may favor the resolution of social dilemmas,
especially if the population density is close to the percolation threshold of
the underlying graph. Regardless of the type of the governing social dilemma as
well as particularities of the interaction graph, we show that under pairwise
imitation the percolation threshold is a universal indicator of how dense the
occupancy ought to be for cooperation to be optimally promoted. We also
demonstrate that myopic updating, due to the lack of efficient spread of
information via imitation, renders the reported mechanism dysfunctional, which
in turn further strengthens its foundations.Comment: 6 two-column pages, 5 figures; accepted for publication in Scientific
Reports [related work available at http://arxiv.org/abs/1205.0541
- …