31 research outputs found
College Student Mental Health: An Evaluation of the DSM-5 Self-Rated Level 1 Cross-Cutting Symptom Measure
© 2018 American Psychological Association. The DSM-5 Self-Rated Level 1 Cross-Cutting Symptom Measure was developed to aid in clinical decision-making for clients seeking psychiatric services and to facilitate empirical investigation of the dimensional nature of mental health issues. Preliminary evidence supports its utility with clinical samples. However, the brief, yet comprehensive structure of the DSM-5 Level 1 measure may benefit a high-risk population that is less likely to seek treatment. College students have high rates of hazardous substance use and co-occurring mental health symptoms, yet rarely seek treatment. Therefore, the current study evaluated the psychometric properties (i.e., construct and criterion-related validity) of the DSM-5 Level 1 measure with a large, diverse sample of non-treatment-seeking college/university students. Data from 7,217 college students recruited from 10 universities in 10 different states across the United States evidenced psychometric validation of the DSM-5 Level 1 measure. Specifically, we found acceptable internal consistency across multi-item DSM-5 domains and moderate to strong correlations among domains (internal validity). Further, several DSM-5 domains were positively associated with longer, validated measures of the same mental health construct and had similar strengths of associations with substance use outcomes compared to longer measures of the same construct (convergent validity). Finally, all DSM-5 domains were negatively associated with self-esteem and positively associated with other theoretically relevant constructs, such as posttraumatic stress (criterion-related validity). Taken together, the DSM-5 Level 1 measure appears to be a viable tool for evaluating psychopathology in college students. Several opportunities for clinical application and empirical investigation of the DSM-5 Level 1 measure are discussed
The prospective memory deficit theory of compulsive checking
Checking compulsions are the most common manifestation of obsessive-compulsive
disorder (OCD), yet the mechanisms which contribute to them are not well understood.
According to one prominent theory — the memory deficit theory — individuals’ compulsions to
check are fueled by a deficit in memory which makes it difficult for them to remember
performing a previous action (e.g., locking a door). The main goal of this dissertation is to
examine the link between memory deficits and checking compulsions. This examination in
carried out in the context of two domains of memory: retrospective memory and prospective
memory.
A review of the literature on memory in OCD shows that previous research on the
memory deficit theory has focused almost exclusively on the domain of retrospective memory,
the ability to remember previously learned information and events. More importantly, the review
demonstrates that deficits in this domain of memory are not unique to checkers and therefore
do not hold the power to explain the compulsion to check. The review further examines the
memory deficit theory in the domain of prospective memory, the ability to remember to carry out
actions (e.g., lock a door). It reviews two of the studies presented in the dissertation which
demonstrate deficits in sub-clinical checkers’ prospective memory and it provides some
supplementary analyses which show that deficits in prospective memory are unique to checkers
and therefore may hold the power to explain the compulsion to check.
Three empirical studies demonstrating that sub-clinical checking compulsions are
associated with subjective and objective deficits in prospective memory comprise the body of
the dissertation. Two of the studies show that the link between checking compulsions and
objective deficits in prospective memory is direct and independent from elevations in
depression, anxiety and distractibility associated with checking compulsions.
The results are used as initial support for the theory that checking compulsions may
develop in part as a compensatory reaction to deficits in prospective memory. If individuals
frequently forget to perform tasks they may develop intrusive doubts about whether they
performed important tasks and when the perceived consequences of a failure are serious these
doubts may lead to checking.Arts, Faculty ofPsychology, Department ofGraduat
The influence of personality and demands of the environment of prospective memory performance
Prospective memory is memory for recollecting intentions, plans, promises, and
agreements. Individuals' performance on tasks requiring prospective memory varies a
great deal. We explored whether some of this variability stems from individual
differences in personality and demands of the environment. As a secondary objective
we explored whether the age-related changes in prospective memory performance that
have previously been attributed to declining cognitive resources can also be explained
by age-related differences in personality and demands of the environment. Participants
were community-dwelling healthy individuals (n = 141) between 18 and 81 years of
age. Participants completed three different prospective memory tasks. Two of these
tasks were lab-based - the intention had to be executed in the laboratory, while the
third was field-based - the intention had to be executed in the context of the
participants' daily life and activities. Participants also completed various indicators of
personality, demands of the environment, and cognitive ability. The results indicated
that personality and demands of the environment reliably predicted who will succeed
and who will fail on all three prospective memory tasks. However, the best predictors of
performance varied across the three prospective memory tasks. Specifically,
conscientiousness predicted performance on the field task and one of the lab tasks
while socially prescribed perfectionism and neuroticism each predicted performance on
one of the lab tasks. Accordingly, the influence of personality and demands of the
environment on the relationship between age and prospective memory performance
also varied across the three tasks. On some prospective memory tasks age-related
differences in personality and demands of the environment compounded with older
adults' declining cognitive resources to impair prospective memory performance while
on other tasks they acted as a partial or a complete buffer against these declining
cognitive resources.Arts, Faculty ofPsychology, Department ofGraduat
Open to Open? An Exploration of Textbook Preferences and Strategies to Offset Textbook Costs for Online Versus On-Campus Students
As open textbook initiatives are on the rise, a burgeoning literature has begun exploring student perceptions of openly licensed textbooks used in higher education. Most of this research has lacked consideration of potential differences in the perceptions of online and on-campus students and has failed to include a control group of students using traditional textbooks. Therefore, the authors employed a 2 x 2 design to directly compare perceptions of online students with on-campus students assigned either open or traditional textbooks. Students (N = 925) enrolled in multiple sections of psychology courses at a midsized R1 institution completed a survey on their perceptions of their particular book’s format and features, as well as strategies they typically employ to offset the cost of expensive course materials. The results revealed that online and on-campus students report disparate strategies for offsetting the high costs of textbooks, different preferences in textbook formats (print versus digital versus both) when cost is not a factor, and differences in their ratings of the importance of various textbook features. Moreover, the results indicate that the use of open textbooks may increase preference for free digital textbooks over paid printed textbooks. Based on these results, the authors suggest that campuses might consider providing customized support to different student populations as open textbook initiatives gain in popularity on university campuses. Additionally, they suggest that prior exposure to open textbooks may increase students’ willingness to use openly licensed materials in future courses. They recommend future research on this question, using a longitudinal within-subjects designs
Measuring cannabis consumption: Psychometric properties of the Daily Sessions, Frequency, Age of Onset, and Quantity of Cannabis Use Inventory (DFAQ-CU)
We created the Daily Sessions, Frequency, Age of Onset, and Quantity of Cannabis Use Inventory (DFAQ-CU) because the current lack of psychometrically sound inventories for measuring these dimensions of cannabis use has impeded research on the effects of cannabis in humans.
A sample of 2,062 cannabis users completed the DFAQ-CU and was used to assess the DFAQ-CU's factor structure and reliability. To assess validity, a subsample of 645 participants completed additional measures of cannabis dependence and problems (Marijuana Smoking History Questionnaire [MSHQ], Timeline Followback [TLFB], Cannabis Abuse Screening Test [CAST], Cannabis Use Disorders Identification Test Revised [CUDIT-R], Cannabis Use Problems Identification Test [CUPIT], and Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test [AUDIT]).
A six-factor structure was revealed, with factors measuring: daily sessions, frequency, age of onset, marijuana quantity, cannabis concentrate quantity, and edibles quantity. The factors were reliable, with Cronbach's alpha coefficients ranging from .69 (daily sessions) to .95 (frequency). Results further provided evidence for the factors' convergent (MSHQ, TLFB), predictive (CAST, CUDIT-R, CUPIT), and discriminant validity (AUDIT).
The DFAQ-CU is the first psychometrically sound inventory for measuring frequency, age of onset, and quantity of cannabis use. It contains pictures of marijuana to facilitate the measurement of quantity of marijuana used, as well as questions to assess the use of different forms of cannabis (e.g., concentrates, edibles), methods of administering cannabis (e.g., joints, hand pipes, vaporizers), and typical THC levels. As such, the DFAQ-CU should help facilitate research on frequency, quantity, and age of onset of cannabis use
Correlations between the factors of the DFAQ-CU.
<p>Correlations between the factors of the DFAQ-CU.</p
Factors loadings for the 24 core items of the DFAQ-CU.
<p>Factors loadings for the 24 core items of the DFAQ-CU.</p