1,486 research outputs found
Mother-child positivity and negativity: Family-wide and child-specific main effects and interactions predict child adjustment
Links between positive and negative aspects of the parent-child relationship and child adjustment are undisputed. Scholars recognize the importance of parental differential treatment (PDT) of siblings, yet, less is known about PDT in the context of the shared (family-wide) parent-child relationship climate, or about the extent to which positivity may buffer children’s adjustment from negativity. Controlling for behavioral stability, we examined the potential for positive and negative parent-child processes to interact across and between child-specific and family-wide levels in the prediction of children’s adjustment. Specifically, in a sample of 2,039 United Kingdom families, we used multilevel models to examine child-specific and family-wide mother-child relationships (at 4 years)—including interactive processes—in the prediction of prosocial behavior and conduct problems (at 7 years). The majority of variance in children’s adjustment resided within-families: siblings were strikingly different. Accounting for behavioral stability, family-wide negativity and negative PDT associated with both prosociality and conduct problems. More important, we demonstrated interactions between, (a) family-wide negativity and negative PDT for conduct problems, as well as, (b) positive and negative PDT in the prediction of both prosocial behavior and conduct problems. Results suggest negative PDT associates with increased conduct problems over time, even when the overall family climate is low in negativity. They also indicate a buffering role of positive PDT on the deleterious effects of negative PDT for children’s adjustment. Implications for both research and practice are discussed, including the importance of information gained by considering more than one child in the family
Child Behavior and Sibling Relationship Quality: A Cross-Lagged Analysis
Bidirectional associations between sibling relationships and children's problem behaviors are robust, and links with prosocial behavior have also been reported. Using cross-lagged models, we were able to conservatively test temporal directions of links between positive and negative aspects of sibling relationships and children's prosocial behavior and conduct problems across a 3-year time span in middle childhood. The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC;
http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary/
) is an ongoing population-based study designed to investigate the effects of a wide range of factors on children's health and development. For the purposes of the current analyses, we included 2,043 ALSPAC families who had just 1 older sibling as well as the target child, with an age gap of no more than 5 years. Mothers reported about the quality of the sibling relationship and both children's prosocial behavior and conduct problems when the target child was 4 years of age and again when the target child was 7 years old. Confirming our hypothesis, individual child behavior was predictive of sibling relationship quality, and sibling relationship quality was predictive of later child behavior, providing robust evidence of bidirectionality for both prosocial behavior and conduct problems. It would be consistent to expect that an improvement in either sibling relationship quality or individual children's behavior could have a positive spill over effect. We also found evidence of older sibling dominance in the domain of prosocial behavior and the positive aspects of sibling interaction
Introducing a Novel Online Observation of Parenting Behavior: Reliability and Validation
OBJECTIVE: Observations of parents with their children are important for better understanding the critical role that parents play in their children’s adjustment, but resource limitations commonly compromise assessment. A novel online observation tool, Etch-a-Sketch Online (ESO), is introduced that allows resource-efficient observations in the family home. DESIGN: Study 1 was a preliminary, cross-sectional study of 20 mothers with their singleton children (M = 5.96 years). Mothers were observed using both ESO (recorded via Skype) and a traditional Etch-a Sketch task recorded during a home-visit; mothers’ positive and negative parenting was coded from these observations. Study 2 was a longitudinal study of 119 mothers and their young twins. Mothers’ ESO-observed positive parenting and negative parenting at Time 1 (M = 5.51 years) were examined as predictors of children’s disruptive behavior at Time 2 (M = 6.04 years) controlling for mothers’ Time 2 self-reported positive and negative parenting. RESULTS: Study 1 provided preliminary evidence of inter-rater reliability and convergent validity of ESO-observations. Study 2 supported this evidence of inter-rater reliability and ESO’s convergent validity as well as providing predictive validity. ESO-observed parenting at Time 1 was associated with children’s disruptive behavior at Time 2, over and above concurrent maternal reports of their own parenting. CONCLUSIONS: ESO shows promise in providing the means for detailed assessment of parenting processes in the home
Mothers' and fathers' perceptions of marital relationships and coparenting twins during school transition
Coparenting and the marital relationship are closely related yet distinct family subsystems hypothesized to influence one another. Little is known about these processes with consideration of more than one child in the family, which may have important implications. Here, we specifically focus on families with young twins, enabling us to account for sibling age-difference confounds, as well as due to the greater parenting demands and higher divorce rates in twin families. Using cross-lagged models for both mothers and fathers, we examined bidirectional associations between coparenting and the marital relationship during children’s transition to formal schooling. Parents of twins from 107 ‘intact’ families reported perceptions of coparenting and the marital relationship via telephone interview at Time 1 (Mchild age = 4years 8 months, SDchild age = 4.44 months) and questionnaire at Time 2 (Mchild age = 6 years, SDchild age = 6.12 months). Accounting for within-time associations and temporal stability for both mothers and fathers, coparenting was positively associated with subsequent reports of the marital relationship; there was no evidence of reciprocal associations between the marital relationship and subsequent coparenting. As children transition to primary school, the quality of coparenting may be a driver of the quality of the marital relationship for parents of twins. Those seeking to improve the marital relationship should pay due attention to perceptions of coparenting
Headspace for parents: qualitative report investigating the use of a mindfulness-based app for managing parents' stress during COVID-19
Background: Stress can compromise parental well-being and may contribute to harsh and critical parenting styles, which are in turn associated with children's conduct problems. Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19)-related restrictions are likely to have exacerbated parental stress as, for many, UK-based family life was altered considerably. Mindfulness has been demonstrated to improve stress management and emotion regulation when delivered to parents in person, however, more accessible online interventions are under-researched. /
Aims: To provide preliminary data on family well-being and parent–child relationships as well as the acceptability and usability of the Headspace app – a self-delivered mindfulness-based intervention – for parents in low-risk families during the early days of the COVID-19 pandemic. /
Method: We provided 12 parents with access to Headspace, and collected qualitative data (semi-structured interviews and 5 minute speech samples) immediately following the initial COVID-19 lockdown in the UK. The resulting transcripts were thematically analysed. /
Results: Most parents reported Headspace to be acceptable and useful – improvements in parents’ own sleep were particularly noted – and there was high adherence to the intervention. However, difficulties related to family well-being and parent–child relationships following the lockdown were also reported. /
Conclusions: As a result of the confounding impact of COVID-19 restrictions, and varied access to app content, we were unable to determine any outcomes to be a result of practising mindfulness specifically. However, COVID-19 has had a profound impact on many UK-based families, including those previously at low risk, and our results demonstrate that Headspace may have beneficial effects for parents. There is a need to more rigorously test this tool with a broader range of families
Early Predictors Of Creative Writing At Age 9
The present study investigated the extent to which creative expressiveness in writing at age 9 could
be predicted by early human figure drawing ability and general cognitive ability, measured at age 4.
Participants (N=277) were members of the Twins Early Development Study, for whom measures of
human figure drawing and general cognitive ability were available at age 4 and a measure of creative
writing was available at age 9. Creativity was measured with the Consensual Assessment Technique,
which is a commonly used technique to estimate creativity of a product. Each story, based on three
pictures shown to children, was coded on 10 dimensions by five independent judges. Creative
Expressiveness score was created as a composite measure of dimensions that correlated highly with
creativity dimension. Human figure drawing ability, measured by The Draw-A-Child test at 4, was found
to be a weak but significant predictor of Creative Expressiveness at age 9 (r =.17). General cognitive
ability at 4 did not predict Creative Expressiveness in writing at 9. It is concluded that examining
individual differences in human figure drawing ability may provide a promising direction for exploring
the early antecedents of creativity throughout childhood
Creative Storytelling In Childhood Is Related To Exam Performance At Age 16
Creativity is only partly recognised in education. A recent meta-analysis estimated a correlation
of r = 0.22 between creativity and educational achievement across many international student samples of
all educational levels. In the meta-analysis, creativity was measured with a variety of measures, including
divergent thinking and remote association tasks. The differences in the measures influenced the strength
of the relationship between creativity and educational achievement. More research is needed to establish
reliable measures of creativity, especially in primary school children, whose creativity remains poorly
evaluated. The present study measured creativity in written stories in children at age 9 using the
Consensual Assessment Technique (CAT). The study employed a longitudinal design, using CAT
creativity scores as a predictor of educational achievement at age 16. Each of the stories from 59 children
were coded by 6 different judges for 10 dimensions, including creativity. The inter-rater reliabilities
between the judges for the 10 dimensions were high (α = .76 - .95). Among the dimensions, a factor
analysis revealed two factors: Creative Expressiveness and Logic. The Creative Expressiveness factor
explained an additional 7 % of variance in English grades, but not in Maths, beyond intelligence, previous
achievement and personality traits associated with creativity. Overall, the study showed that CAT is a
robust and reliable measure to detect verbal creativity in childhood. The results also suggest that early
creativity predicts later academic achievement, calling for more attention to early creativity assessment
and development
- …