69 research outputs found

    The topography of 4 subtraction ERP-waveforms derived from a 3-tone auditory oddball task in healthy young adults

    Get PDF
    Introduction: Five components were studied in 4 subtraction waveforms derived from ERPs obtained in passive and active conditions of a 3-tone oddball task (common=70%, C, 0.8 KHz; deviant=15%, D, 2 KHz; 1.4 KHz=15%, t, also used as a target (T)). These waveforms reflect different stimulus-mismatch processes and thus their topography could be revealing of different brain regions mediating them. Methods: The following mismatches were studied: stimulus-mismatch (deviant - common, D/C, rarity and pitch confounded, known as the mismatch negativity, MMN), pitch-mismatch (T - deviant, T/D, rarity not target features controlled, known as processing negativity PN), attention - mismatch (T - t), T/t, controlled for pitch and rarity to show the influence of target features, known as the Negative difference Nd). These are compared with Goodin's procedure (G-wv, (T-common (active) - (t-common (passive)- the "Goodin-waveform"). Results: There were main site effects in normalized data in all cases (not P2 and N2 latency). There were separate frontal and posterior contributions to P1, with the former emphasized where target comparisons were involved. Frontal N1 peaks, largest in D/C (MMN), spread posterior and to the right where target matching was involved. P2 posterior maxima were also less localized where target features were involved in the comparison. N2 topography was similar between waveforms but spread slightly more to each side in the T/t comparison (i.e. Nd). Onset was earlier in the D/C comparison (i.e MMN). Parietal P3 peaks in waves based on target-ERPs showed a left temporal shift (vs D/C), though in T/D P3 was in fact maximal on the right (i.e. PN waveform). Conclusions: Thus an attentional effect(controlled processing) is evident as early as 60 ms. Target features modify the anteroposterior distribution of positivity and negativity for the early components and in the lateralization of P3-like positivity. A comparison of waveforms by latency of potential shift (running t-test) vs. peak identification (MANOVA) is illustrated and discussed. D/C (MMN) and T/t (Nd) waveforms, rather than T/D or G-wv (PN & Goodin waveforms) waveforms are recommended for distinguishing comparator mechanisms for stimulus- and task-relevant features

    Development and topography of auditory event-related potentials (ERPs): Mismatch and processing negativity in individuals 8-22 years of age

    Get PDF
    Introduction: How do ERPs reflecting auditory information processing develop across adolescence? This development is described for the amplitude and latency of five ERP components and four difference-waves in four groups of 11 healthy subjects with mean ages of 10, 14, 17 & 21 years. Methods: Vector normalised data were recorded from 19 sites during diffuse and focused attention in a three-tone oddball. (i.e. in passive, diffuse-attention and active, focussed-attention, discrimination conditions) to see how ERP loci varied with age for tone-type, attention-condition and for four types of difference-wave reflecting nontarget and target comparisons: (mismatch negativity, MMN, an auditory working memory trace; Negative difference, Nd, an attentional trace, but also Processing Negativity, PN and the Goodin-waveform). Results: Age interacted with site for most components. P1 loci sensitive to rare tones moved posteriorly and N1 loci lost their right bias at early puberty. But P2 loci did not move anterior to Cz until adulthood. N2 amplitude, sensitive to attention condition, developed a mature frontal focus by 17 years. Right-biased P3 loci move to the midline with focused attention in young and old alike. Difference-waves reflected three developmental stages (figure 2): In 10 year-olds early deflections (<150 ms) were diffusely distributed; in mid-adolescence the main frontal negative component (150-300 ms) became well-formed and lost an earlier right bias; over 17 years the late positive complex developed a right bias in target-derived waves. Latency decreases for early frontal components were marked in 10-14y olds and for later posterior components in 14-17y olds. MMN topography matured (from a right lateral to bilateral distribution) between 10 and 14 y, while Nd topography matured and became bilateral between 14 and 17y. Conclusions: Major developments of brain function appear at the onset of adolescence (<14y) in early stimulus-selection processes and during adolescence in the differential use of this information (N2- and P3-like latencies

    Impaired attention-dependent augmentation of MMN in nonparanoid vs paranoid schizophrenic patients: a comparison with obsessive-compulsive disorder and healthy subjects

    Get PDF
    Introduction: Mismatch negativity (MMN), in the deviant-minus-standard event-related potential (ERP) difference-waveform, may represent a working memory trace for tone differences (a deviant among a sequence of standards). Most, but not all studies find MMN reduced in patients with schizophrenia. Aims: This report investigates if differences may be attributable to experimental condition (diffuse vs focused attention), component identification (N1-like vs N2-like), topographic distribution and clinical condition (with / without paranoid -hallucinatory symptoms, PH/NP). Comparisons were made for 12 PH and 12 NP schizophrenic patients with 13 obsessive compulsive and 25 normal control subjects. Results: Frontal MMN reduction in schizophrenics largely resulted from an absence of an increase in focused attention conditions as in comparison groups. But there was marked activity recorded from sites over the temporal lobe in NP patients. These features were not reflected in other components except for a visible but nonsignifiant N1-like temporal locus in NP patients. Further, schizophrenic patients did not show an increase in late positivity with focused attention like the comparison groups. Conclusions: The results show that so-called automatic processing deficits in schizophrenia (amount and locus of MMN) are best seen in situations requiring the activation of controlled attentional processes. It is suggested that impaired processing of irrelevant stimuli and reduced frontal MMN in NP patients may reflect reduced dopaminergic responsivity

    Auditory event-related potential (ERP) and difference-wave topography in schizophrenic patients with/without active hallucinations and deluisions: a comparison with young obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and healthy subjects

    Get PDF
    Introduction: Event-related potentials (ERPs) in schizophrenics have been reported to show a reduced P3 on the left and less frontal mismatch negativity. But the specificity of such findings to component, its locus, the type of eliciting event and patient group remains uncertain. Hence, we examined ERP topography for P3, N2 and 3 precursor peaks according to stimulus (3-tone oddball), attention condition (diffuse/focused) and four types of difference-waves (Mismatch negativity, Processing negativity, Negative difference (Nd) and the 'Goodin-Waveform'). Method: We contrasted ERPs in a 3-tone oddball task-form in 24 healthy (mean 18.5y of age) and 13 OCD (mean age 16.3 y) subjects with schizophrenic patients with high versus low ratings of active delusions and hallucinations (12 paranoid-hallucinatory, PH [mean age 18.5y]; 12 nonparanoid, NP [mean age 18.9y]) Results: 1. P3 peaks were delayed and reduced in NP and PH groups. Peaks in the midline were usual in the focused attention condition, but a right bias in diffuse attention (passive presentation). 2. P3 responses to irrelevant non-targets remained lateralised in NP, and small in OCD patients. All showed a small left and anterior bias in the P3-like peak recorded after subtraction in the difference waves. 3. Mismatch negativity (MMN) peaks shifted to the right in OCD, laterally to both sides in PH and more posteriorly in NP patients. 4. Frontal processing negativity was biased to the left (early) in NP and to the right (late) in PH groups. 5. Early peak topography in the difference waveforms reflected some of these later changes (e.g., for PH and NP groups the normal right bias in the P1-like peak was absent; the N1-like peak was reduced and widely distributed: for the NP group, the P2-like peak appeared smaller on the left). 6. In OCD patients, the peak latencies were topographically undifferentiated for P1 and P2, or delayed in the case of the N2 component. Conclusions: A) The OCD group showed an unusual regional allocation of processing effort. B) Before 200 ms, fronto-central activity was more widespread in both the PH and NP groups. C) NP patients, in particular, treated irrelevant stimuli anomalously. D) Lateralisation of negativity in target- and nontarget-derived difference waves may reflect differential disruption of the frontal-temporal dialogue in registering important vs. unimportant features. Indeed, the apparent left/right differences of negative difference (Nd) or processing negativity amplitude may not so much reflect amplitude differences as a delayed latency over left frontal areas in PH and over the right frontal areas in NP patients with schizophrenia

    Event-related potentials during an auditory discrimination with prepulse inhibition in patients with schizophrenia, obsessive-compulsive disorder and healthy subjects

    Get PDF
    Introduction: Prepulse inhibition (PPI) is a measure of the influence of a stimulus (S1) on the response elicited by a second stimulus (S2) occurring shortly afterwards. Most S1/S2 measures of gating have used behavioral startle and the P50 event-related potential (ERP) amplitudes to detect PPI in a simple paired stimulus paradigm. Aims: Here we report on two behavioral (EOG and reaction time, RT) and 5 ERP measures of PPI where S2 was the target or standard in an oddball discrimination. Subjects were 21 healthy controls (CON), 11 obsessive-compulsive (OCD) and 9 schizophrenic patients (SCH). Results: The prepulse 100ms before S2 induced fewer omission errors and longer RTs compared to a 500ms S1-S2 interval in all subjects. PPI was also evident in EOG, P50, N1, P3 but not P2 or N2 amplitudes of CON subjects. SCH patients showed attenuation of PPI on the same measures. OCD patients were characterized only by their slow RT and a marginal attenuation of EOG-PPI. Discussion: A correlational analysis implied separate relationships of ERP indices of PPI to the cognitive and psychomotor consequences of the prepulse on behavioral and discrimination responses. However SCH patients showed a general rather than a specific impairment of these indices

    A left temporal lobe impairment of auditory information processing in schizophrenia: an event-related potential study

    Get PDF
    Introduction: A measure of auditory prepulse inhibition (PPI) or sensory gating is the reduction of the scalp-recorded P1 event-related potential (ERP) after a sound that is preceded by 100-300 ms by a click as prepulse. This measure of sensory gating was adapted to study the effect of a prepulse on processing tones that were part of a "Go/no-go" discrimination. Methods: ERPs were recorded at right and left frontal and temporal sites on the scalp of groups of patients with schizophrenia (SCH, 8), obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD, 10) and healthy controls (CON, 19). Results: a) Both patient groups responded slower and showed more errors of omission than controls. b) A prepulse presented 100 ms (but not at 500 ms) before either tone reduced the P1 or P50 ERP amplitude on healthy controls, but showed a right temporal shift in the SCH patients. c) If the tone was the 'no-go' stimulus in the tone discrimination the prepulse reduced the N1 amplitude in both normal controls and SCH patients. This N100 amplitude was similar in records from over the left and right hemisphere of controls but was shifted to right temporal sites in the SCH group. d) OCD patients showed a relative predominance of the left hemisphere in the gating of frontal P1, N100 and P300 components, but early gating was not significantly impaired in this group. Conclusions: These results show a reduction of a PPI-like effect on early processing (e.g. P50/P1) that is more marked in the left hemisphere of SCH patients, and may affect channel selection for processing information about task-relevant sounds (e.g. N100

    Challenges for Coring Deep Permafrost on Earth and Mars

    Get PDF
    This is the published version. Final publication is available from Mary Ann Liebert, Inc., publishers http://www.dx.doi.org/10.1089/ast.2007.0159.A scientific drilling expedition to the High Lake region of Nunavut, Canada, was recently completed with the goals of collecting samples and delineating gradients in salinity, gas composition, pH, pe, and microbial abundance in a 400 m thick permafrost zone and accessing the underlying pristine subpermafrost brine. With a triple-barrel wireline tool and the use of stringent quality assurance and quality control (QA/QC) protocols, 200 m of frozen, Archean, mafic volcanic rock was collected from the lower boundary that separates the permafrost layer and subpermafrost saline water. Hot water was used to remove cuttings and prevent the drill rods from freezing in place. No cryopegs were detected during penetration through the permafrost. Coring stopped at the 535 m depth, and the drill water was bailed from the hole while saline water replaced it. Within 24 hours, the borehole iced closed at 125 m depth due to vapor condensation from atmospheric moisture and, initially, warm water leaking through the casing, which blocked further access. Preliminary data suggest that the recovered cores contain viable anaerobic microorganisms that are not contaminants even though isotopic analyses of the saline borehole water suggests that it is a residue of the drilling brine used to remove the ice from the upper, older portion of the borehole. Any proposed coring mission to Mars that seeks to access subpermafrost brine will not only require borehole stability but also a means by which to generate substantial heating along the borehole string to prevent closure of the borehole from condensation of water vapor generated by drilling. Astrobiology 8, 623–638

    Novelty-elicited mismatch negativity in patients with schizophrenia on admission and discharge.

    No full text
    OBJECTIVE: Given recent reports of differences between mismatch negativity (MMN) elicited by always novel sounds (novelty-elicited MMN) and that elicited by repeated rare deviants (conventional MMN), we investigated novelty-elicited MMN and P3a in patients with schizophrenia before and after a nonstandardized inpatient treatment. DESIGN: Electrophysiological and clinical assessment of patients on admission and discharge from hospital. Assessment of control subjects on 2 sessions. SETTING: Inpatient treatment in a psychiatric university hospital. SUBJECTS: 20 patients with schizophrenia and 21 healthy control subjects of similar age and sex. Selection of patients with first- to third-episode schizophrenia. OUTCOME MEASURES: Early and late component MMN amplitudes and latencies, P3a amplitudes and latencies, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS), Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF), Extrapyramidal Symptom Scale (EPS), Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale (AIMS) and chlorpromazine equivalents. RESULTS: In patients with schizophrenia, novelty-elicited MMN was unimpaired on admission, and there was a statistically significant reduction of the late MMN component with treatment. Improvements in symptom expression were associated with increased latencies of the early MMN component. CONCLUSION: Results indicate differences in information processing between conventional and novelty-elicited MMN. Some components of the novelty-elicited MMN might be more state dependent than those of the conventional MMN
    • …
    corecore