8 research outputs found

    Supplementary Materials "How we think about numbers - Early counting and mathematical abstraction" - Chapter 1

    No full text
    Supplementary Materials for Chapter 1 of the Doctoral Dissertation "How we think about numbers - Early counting and mathematical abstraction". Contains Preregistration, open data and codebook</p

    How we think about numbers: early counting and mathematical abstraction

    No full text
    As children learn to count, they make one of their first mathematical abstractions. They initially learn how numbers in the count sequence correspond to quantities of physical things if the rules of counting are followed (i.e., if you say the numbers in order ā€œone two three four ā€¦ā€ as you tag each thing with a number). Around the age of four-years-old, children discover that these rules also define numbers in relation to each other, such that numbers contain meaning in themselves and without reference to the physical world (e.g., ā€œfiveā€ is ā€œoneā€ more than ā€œfourā€). It is through learning to count, that children discover the natural numbers as mathematical symbols defined by abstract rules.In this dissertation, I explored the developmental trajectory and the cognitive mechanisms of how we gain an understanding of the natural numbers as children. I present new methodological, empirical, and theoretical insights on how and when in the process of learning to count, children discover that numbers represent cardinalities, that numbers can be defined in relation to each other by the successor function and that numbers refer to units. Lastly, I explore this mathematical abstraction as the foundation of how we think about numbers as adults.My work critically tested prominent theories on how learning to count gives meaning to numbers through analogical mapping and conceptual bootstrapping. Findings across five empirical studies suggest that the process is more gradual and continuous than previous theories have proposed. Children begin to understand numbers as cardinalities defined in relation to other numbers by the successor function before they fully grasp the rules of counting. With learning the rules of counting this understanding continuously expands and matures. I further suggest that children may only fully understand numbers as abstract mathematical symbols once they understand how counting and numbers refer to the abstract notion of units rather than to physical things.The central finding of this dissertation is that learning to count does not change childrenā€™s understanding of numbers altogether and all at once. Nonetheless, when learning to count, children accomplish a fascinating mathematical abstraction, which builds the foundation for lifelong mathematical learning.</p

    Finding the subitizing in groupitizing: Evidence for parallel subitizing of dots and groups in grouped arrays

    No full text
    ā€˜Groupitizingā€™ refers to the observation that visually grouped arrays can be accurately enumerated much faster than can unstructured arrays. Previous research suggests that visual grouping allows participants to draw on arithmetic abilities and possibly use mental calculations to enumerate grouped arrays quickly and accurately. Here, we address how subitizing might be involved in finding the operands for mental calculations in grouped dot arrays. We investigated whether participants can use multiple subitizing processes to enumerate both the number of dots and the number of groups in a grouped array. We found that these multiple subitizing processes can take place within 150 ms and that dots and groups seem to be subitized in parallel and with equal priority. Implications for research on mechanisms of groupitizing are discussed.<br

    Beyond Representation: Open Data, Materials, and Code

    No full text
    Open data, materials and code for the manuscript "Beyond Representations: Why young children count wrong units".Two studies are included as separate zip files with all data for each study within the relevant folder.</div

    Counting many as one: Young children can understand sets as units except when counting

    No full text
    Young children frequently make a peculiar counting mistake. When asked to count units that are sets of multiple items, such as the number of families at a party, they often count discrete items (i.e., individual people) rather than the number of sets (i.e., families). One explanation concerns childrenā€™s incomplete understanding of what constitutes a unit, resulting in a preference for discrete items. Here, we demonstrate that childrenā€™s incomplete understanding of counting also plays a role. In an experiment with 4- to 5-year-old children (N = 43), we found that even if children are able to name sets, group items into sets, and create one-to-one correspondences with sets, many children are nevertheless unable to count sets as units. We conclude that a nascent understanding of the abstraction principle of counting is also a cause of some childrenā€™s counting errors.</p

    Data for Iconicity in mathematical notation

    No full text
    Data and experimental materials associated with the paper "Iconicity in mathematical notation"

    Iconicity in mathematical notation: commutativity and symmetry

    No full text
    Mathematical notation includes a vast array of signs. Most mathematical signs appear to be symbolic, in the sense that their meaning is arbitrarily related to their visual appearance. We explored the hypothesis that mathematical signs with iconic aspects ā€“ those which visually resemble in some way the concepts they represent ā€“ offer a cognitive advantage over those which are purely symbolic. An early formulation of this hypothesis was made by Christine Ladd in 1883 who suggested that symmetrical signs should be used to convey commutative relations, because they visually resemble the mathematical concept they represent. Two controlled experiments provide the first empirical test of, and evidence for, Laddā€™s hypothesis. In Experiment 1 we find that participants are more likely to attribute commutativity to operations denoted by symmetric signs. In Experiment 2 we further show that using symmetric signs as notation for commutative operations can increase mathematical performance

    Data from an international multi-centre study of statistics and mathematics anxieties and related variables in university students (the SMARVUS dataset)

    No full text
    This large, international dataset contains survey responses from N = 12,570 students from 100 universities in 35 countries, collected in 21 languages. We measured anxieties (statistics, mathematics, test, trait, social interaction, performance, creativity, intolerance of uncertainty, and fear of negative evaluation), self-efficacy, persistence, and the cognitive reflection test, and collected demographics, previous mathematics grades, self-reported and official statistics grades, and statistics module details. Data reuse potential is broad, including testing links between anxieties and statistics/mathematics education factors, and examining instrumentsā€™ psychometric properties across different languages and contexts. Data and metadata are stored on the Open Science Framework website [https://osf.io/mhg94/]
    corecore