759 research outputs found

    What Fraction of Sun-like Stars have Planets?

    Full text link
    The radial velocities of ~1800 nearby Sun-like stars are currently being monitored by eight high-sensitivity Doppler exoplanet surveys. Approximately 90 of these stars have been found to host exoplanets massive enough to be detectable. Thus at least ~5% of target stars possess planets. If we limit our analysis to target stars that have been monitored the longest (~15 years), ~11% possess planets. If we limit our analysis to stars monitored the longest and whose low surface activity allow the most precise velocity measurements, ~25% possess planets. By identifying trends of the exoplanet mass and period distributions in a sub-sample of exoplanets less-biased by selection effects, and linearly extrapolating these trends into regions of parameter space that have not yet been completely sampled, we find at least ~9% of Sun-like stars have planets in the mass and orbital period ranges Msin(i) > 0.3 M_Jupiter and P 0.1 M_Jupiter and P < 60 years. Even this larger area of the mass-period plane is less than 20% of the area occupied by our planetary system, suggesting that this estimate is still a lower limit to the true fraction of Sun-like stars with planets, which may be as large as ~100%.Comment: Conforms to version accepted by ApJ. Color version and movie available at http://bat.phys.unsw.edu.au/~charley/download/whatfrac

    Differential effects of Alzheimer\u27s disease and Huntington\u27s disease on the performance of mental rotation

    Get PDF
    he ability to spatially rotate a mental image was compared in patients with Alzheimer\u27s disease (AD; n = 18) and patients with Huntington\u27s disease (HD; n = 18). Compared to their respective age-matched normal control (NC) group, the speed, but not the accuracy, of mental rotation abnormally decreased with increasing angle of orientation for patients with HD. In contrast, the accuracy, but not the speed, of rotation abnormally decreased with increasing angle of orientation for patients with AD. Additional analyses showed that these unique patterns of performance were not attributable to different speed/accuracy trade-off sensitivities. This double dissociation suggests that the distinct brain regions affected in the two diseases differentially contribute to speed and accuracy of mental rotation. Specifically, the slowing exhibited by HD patients may be mediated by damage to the basal ganglia, whereas the spatial manipulation deficit of AD patients may reflect pathology in parietal and temporal lobe association cortices important for visuospatial processing. (JINS, 2005, 11, 30–39.

    Study Smarter, Not Harder

    Get PDF
    Provides tips for studying

    Repeated Intracarotid Amobarbital Tests

    Get PDF
    Rationale:Our goal was to determine the frequency of repeated intracarotid amobarbital test (IAT) at our center and to estimate the retest reliability of the IAT for both language and memory lateralization. Methods: A total of 1,249 consecutive IATs on 1,190 patients were retrospectively reviewed for repeat tests. Results: In 4% of patients the IAT was repeated in order to deliver satisfactory information on either language or memory lateralization. Reasons for repetition included obtundation and inability to test for memory lateralization, inability to test for language lateralization, no hemiparesis during first test, no aphasia during first test, atypical vessel filling, and bleeding complications from the catheter insertion site. Language lateralization was reproduced in all but one patient. Repeated memory test results were less consistent across tests, and memory lateralization was unreliable in 63% of the patients. Discussion: In spite of test limitations by a varying dose of amobarbital, crossover of amobarbital from one side to the other, testing of both hemispheres on the same day, practice effects, unblinded observers, fluctuating cooperation of the patients, and a biased sample of patients language lateralization was reproduced in all but one patient. In contrast, repeated memory test results were frequently contradictory. Memory results on IAT therefore seem much less robust than the results of language testing. Gain of reliable information versus the risks of complications and failed tests has to be considered when a patient is subjected to an IAT

    Expectations about Memory Change Across the Life Span Are Impacted By Aging Stereotypes.

    Get PDF
    This study examined whether expectations about memory change with age vary for different personality types. Four adjectives from each of Hummert’s age-stereotype trait sets were selected to create 11 adjective clusters varying in both valence (positive versus negative) and relevance to memory functioning. Three hundred and seventy three participants in three age groups rated the memory abilities of target adults, defined by the adjective clusters, across the adult life span. Consistent with past studies, participants believed in age-related memory decline. However, participants rated target adults with positive personality traits as having better memory ability and less age-related memory decline than target adults with negative personality traits. This effect was larger when the traits were relevant to memory than when they were not. Finally, older participants were more strongly influenced by both the valence and the relevance of the personality descriptions than younger participants

    Young and Older Adults’ Beliefs about Effective Ways to Mitigate Age-Related Memory Decline

    Get PDF
    This study investigated whether young and older adults vary in their beliefs about the impact of various mitigating factors on age-related memory decline. Eighty young (ages 18–23) and eighty older (ages 60–82) participants reported their beliefs about their own memory abilities and the strategies that they use in their everyday lives to attempt to control their memory. Participants also reported their beliefs about memory change with age for hypothetical target individuals who were described as using (or not using) various means to mitigate memory decline. There were no age differences in personal beliefs about control over current or future memory ability. However, the two age groups differed in the types of strategies they used in their everyday life to control their memory. Young adults were more likely to use internal memory strategies, whereas older adults were more likely to focus on cognitive exercise and maintaining physical health as ways to optimize their memory ability. There were no age differences in rated memory change across the life span in hypothetical individuals. Both young and older adults perceived strategies related to improving physical and cognitive health as effective means of mitigating memory loss with age, whereas internal memory strategies were perceived as less effective means for controlling agerelated memory decline

    Memory Performance is Related to Language Dominance as Determined by the intracarotid amobarbital procedure

    Get PDF
    Objective The goal of this study was to explore the relationship between language and memory lateralization in patients with epilepsy undergoing the intracarotid amobarbital procedure. Methods In 386 patients, language lateralization and memory lateralization as determined by laterality index (LI) were correlated with each other. Results Language lateralization and memory lateralization were positively correlated (r = 0.34, P \u3c 0.01). Correlations differed depending on the presence and type of lesion (χ2 = 7.98, P \u3c 0.05). LIs correlated significantly higher (z = 2.82, P \u3c 0.05) in patients with cortical dysplasia (n = 41, r = 0.61, P \u3c 0.01) compared with the group without lesions (n = 90, r = 0.16, P \u3e 0.05), with patients with hippocampal sclerosis falling between these two groups. Both memory (P \u3c 0.01) and language (P \u3c 0.01) LIs were higher in right- compared with left-sided lesions. Conclusion Correlation of language and memory is more pronounced in patients with structural lesions as compared with patients without lesions on MRI

    The Cosmic Coincidence as a Temporal Selection Effect Produced by the Age Distribution of Terrestrial Planets in the Universe

    Full text link
    The energy densities of matter and the vacuum are currently observed to be of the same order of magnitude: (Ωm0≈0.3)∼(ΩΛ0≈0.7)(\Omega_{m 0} \approx 0.3) \sim (\Omega_{\Lambda 0} \approx 0.7). The cosmological window of time during which this occurs is relatively narrow. Thus, we are presented with the cosmological coincidence problem: Why, just now, do these energy densities happen to be of the same order? Here we show that this apparent coincidence can be explained as a temporal selection effect produced by the age distribution of terrestrial planets in the Universe. We find a large (∼68\sim 68 %) probability that observations made from terrestrial planets will result in finding Ωm\Omega_m at least as close to ΩΛ\Omega_{\Lambda} as we observe today. Hence, we, and any observers in the Universe who have evolved on terrestrial planets, should not be surprised to find Ωm∼ΩΛ\Omega_m \sim \Omega_{\Lambda}. This result is relatively robust if the time it takes an observer to evolve on a terrestrial planet is less than ∼10\sim 10 Gyr.Comment: Submitted to Ap

    Wada Test Reliability (Response to Haber et al.)

    Get PDF
    Response piece
    • …
    corecore