981 research outputs found
Perception of English /i/ and /I/ by Japanese and Spanish Listeners: Longitudinal Results
Flege’s Speech Learning Model predicts that if an L2 learner perceives an L2 speech sound as similar to an L1 speech sound, the two sounds will be combined as a diaphone category, the properties of which will eventually be intermediate between the properties of the L1 and L2 sound. In contrast if the L2 sound is perceived as new, then a new category will be established with properties which may eventually match the properties of the L2 sound. Canadian English has two high front vowels: tense /i/ and lax /I/ differing in spectral and duration properties. Japanese has two high front vowels: long /i:/ and short /i/ differing in duration only. English /i/ and /I/ are expected to be perceived as similar to Japanese /i:/ and /i/, and Japanese learners of English are predicted to
establish diaphone categories. Their identification of English /i/ and /I/ is predicted to initially match their perception of Japanese /i:/ and /i/, but eventually be intermediate between the native norms for the L1 and L2 categories. Spanish has one high front
vowel. Spanish learners of English are predicted to perceive English /I/ as less similar
to Spanish /i/ than English /i/, and are predicted to eventually establish a new /i:/
category. Their identification of English /i/ and /I/ is predicted to initially be poor but
eventually match that of English listeners. These predictions were tested using a
multidimensional edited-speech continuum covering the English words /bIt bit bId bid/.
Properties which varied in the continuum included vowel spectral properties and vowel
duration. A longitudinal study was conducted testing Japanese and Spanish speaking learners of English one month and six months after their arrival in Canada. Japanese listeners were found to have a primarily duration-based categorical boundary between English /i/ and /I/ which did not change between the initial and final tests. Spanish listeners did not have a categorical identification pattern in the initial test, but they did establish duration-based or spectrally-based categorical boundaries by the time of the
final test. Results were therefore consistent with the theoretical predictions
An acoustic and statistical analysis of Spanish mid-vowel allophones
In 1918 Navarro Tomás claimed that Spanish mid vowels have open and close allophones. Whilst some acoustic studies have supported these claims, others have not. The conclusions in these studies were subjective since they failed to apply statistical analysis to the acoustic data. Statistical analysis is required to determine whether data with a range of values can be grouped into distinct categories. The present study investigates the production of Spanish /e/ and /o/ in several contexts in which Navarro Tomás claimed open and close allophones would be found. Recordings were made of a male and a female speaker of educated Madrid Spanish, the dialect which Navarro Tomás originally described. First and second formants were measured and statistically analysed. Formant frequencies did not cluster into two groups associated with the contexts in which Navarro Tomás claimed each allophone would occur. Other potential allophones were identified: fronted and retracted allophones for /o/; and close-fronted, central, and openretracted allophones for /e/.In 1918 Navarro Tomás claimed that Spanish mid vowels have open and close allophones. Whilst some acoustic studies have supported these claims, others have not. The conclusions in these studies were subjective since they failed to apply statistical analysis to the acoustic data. Statistical analysis is required to determine whether data with a range of values can be grouped into distinct categories. The present study investigates the production of Spanish /e/ and /o/ in several contexts in which Navarro Tomás claimed open and close allophones would be found. Recordings were made of a male and a female speaker of educated Madrid Spanish, the dialect which Navarro Tomás originally described. First and second formants were measured and statistically analysed. Formant frequencies did not cluster into two groups associated with the contexts in which Navarro Tomás claimed each allophone would occur. Other potential allophones were identified: fronted and retracted allophones for /o/; and close-fronted, central, and openretracted allophones for /e/.En 1918 Navarro Tomás afirmó que había alófonos abiertos y cerrados de las vocales medias de castellano. Aunque los resultados de varios estudios subsiguientes están de acuerdo con la afirmación, otros están en desacuerdo. Las conclusiones de estos estudios son subjetivas porque no analizaron estadísticamente los datos acústicos. Un análisis estadístico es imprescindible para determinar si se pueden asignar datos con una variedad de valores a categorías distintas. El presente estudio investiga la producción de /e/ y /o/ castellano en varios de los contextos en que Navarro Tomás afirmó que había alófonos abiertos y cerrados. Se grabaron las producciones de un hombre y una mujer, hablantes de un dialecto madrileño educado, el dialecto que Navarro Tomás describió. Se medió y analizó estadísticamente el primer y el segundo formante de las vocales. Las frecuencias de los formantes no se apiñaron en dos grupos asociados con los contextos en que Navarro Tomás afirmó aparecía cada alófono. Se identificaron otros alófonos potenciales: anterior y posterior para /o/; y anterior-cerrado, central, y posterior-abierto para /e/
The impact in forensic voice comparison of lack of calibration and of mismatched conditions between the known-speaker recording and the relevant-population sample recordings
In a 2017 New South Wales case, a forensic practitioner conducted a forensic voice comparison using a Gaussian mixture model – universal background model (GMM-UBM). The practitioner did not report the results of empirical tests of the performance of this system under conditions reflecting those of the case under investigation. The practitioner trained the model for the numerator of the likelihood ratio using the known-speaker recording, but trained the model for the denominator of the likelihood ratio (the UBM) using high-quality audio recordings, not recordings which reflected the conditions of the known-speaker recording. There was therefore a difference in the mismatch between the numerator model and the questioned-speaker recording versus the mismatch between the denominator model and the questioned-speaker recording. In addition, the practitioner did not calibrate the output of the system. The present paper empirically tests the performance of a replication of the practitioner’s system. It also tests a system in which the UBM was trained on known-speaker-condition data and which was empirically calibrated. The performance of the former system was very poor, and the performance of the latter was substantially better
What should a forensic practitioner's likelihood ratio be? II
In the debate as to whether forensic practitioners should assess and report the precision of the strength of evidence statements that they report to the courts, I remain unconvinced by proponents of the position that only a subjectivist concept of probability is legitimate. I consider this position counterproductive for the goal of having forensic practitioners implement, and courts not only accept but demand, logically correct and scientifically valid evaluation of forensic evidence. In considering what would be the best approach for evaluating strength of evidence, I suggest that the desiderata be (1) to maximise empirically demonstrable performance; (2) to maximise objectivity in the sense of maximising transparency and replicability, and minimising the potential for cognitive bias; and (3) to constrain and make overt the forensic practitioner’s subjective-judgement based decisions so that the appropriateness of those decisions can be debated before the judge in an admissibility hearing and/or before the trier of fact at trial. All approaches require the forensic practitioner to use subjective judgement, but constraining subjective judgement to decisions relating to selection of hypotheses, properties to measure, training and test data to use, and statistical modelling procedures to use – decisions which are remote from the output stage of the analysis – will substantially reduce the potential for cognitive bias. Adopting procedures based on relevant data, quantitative measurements, and statistical models, and directly reporting the output of the statistical models will also maximise transparency and replicability. A procedure which calculates a Bayes factor on the basis of relevant sample data and reference priors is no less objective than a frequentist calculation of a likelihood ratio on the same data. In general, a Bayes factor calculated using uninformative or reference priors will be closer to a value of 1 than a frequentist best estimate likelihood ratio. The bound closest to 1 based on a frequentist best estimate likelihood ratio and an assessment of its precision will also, by definition, be closer to a value of 1 than the frequentist best estimate likelihood ratio. From a practical perspective, both procedures shrink the strength of evidence value towards the neutral value of 1. A single-value Bayes factor or likelihood ratio may be easier for the courts to handle than a distribution. I therefore propose as a potential practical solution, the use of procedures which account for imprecision by shrinking the calculated Bayes factor or likelihood ratio towards 1, the choice of the particular procedure being based on empirical demonstration of performance
A response to: "NIST experts urge caution in use of courtroom evidence presentation method"
A press release from the National Institute of Standards and Technology
(NIST)could potentially impede progress toward improving the analysis of
forensic evidence and the presentation of forensic analysis results in courts
in the United States and around the world. "NIST experts urge caution in use of
courtroom evidence presentation method" was released on October 12, 2017, and
was picked up by the phys.org news service. It argues that, except in
exceptional cases, the results of forensic analyses should not be reported as
"likelihood ratios". The press release, and the journal article by NIST
researchers Steven P. Lund & Harri Iyer on which it is based, identifies some
legitimate points of concern, but makes a strawman argument and reaches an
unjustified conclusion that throws the baby out with the bathwater
Revolution, Modernization and Nation-Building in Diem’s Vietnam: Civic Action, 1955-1963
Using archival material from the Socialist Republic of Vietnam this dissertation demonstrates that from 1954 to 1963 the South Vietnamese government of President Ngô Binh Diçm attempted to promote its own national revolution to build a modem, independent Southeast Asian nation. This revolution, I argue, was intended to rival the socialist revolution being fomented by the communists in North Vietnam and was consistent with the efforts of other Southeast Asian leaders who were attempting to create viable, independent national polities within a region that was subject to the twin forces of decolonization and the Cold War. Beginning in early 1955 the Special Commissariat for Civic Action became an important vehicle through which Diçm intended to realize his revolution. During this period the Special Commissariat for Civic Action faced a number of obstacles in fulfilling its mission, the most significant being a communist-led insurgency. American policymakers in both the Eisenhower and Kennedy Administrations shared the Diçm government’s belief that the best means to counter this insurgency lay in building a viable nation in South Vietnam. The dissertation contends that the developmental model American policymakers in Washington and advisors in Vietnam based their nation- building prescriptions upon was rooted in an idealized conception of their own nation’s evolutionary trajectory and completely ignored any local realities. Such obtuseness, I argue, offended die nationalist sensibilities of the Diçm government leading it to exclude American advisors in Sài Gôn from deliberations over the development of the Civic
Action program, demonstrating there were significant limits to the scope of American influence in South Vietnam. The thesis concludes that the efforts of the Civic Action cadres to win peasant support to the Diçm government were undermined by a combination of internal contradictions in the revolution they were attempting to foment, poor policy formulation and a heavy-handed campaign to eliminate subversive elements. I argue that these phenomena not only prevented the revolutionary ideals of the Diçm government from resonating with the rural population, but they actually contributed to the rise of the communist-led Nationalist Front for the Liberation of South Vietnam in late 1960. Keywords: nation-building, nationalism, liberal-developmentalism, modernization, modernization theory, Orientalism, postcolonialism, First Republic of Vietnam, Civic Action, United States-South Vietnamese Relation
Admissibility of forensic voice comparison testimony in England and Wales
In 2015 the Criminal Practice Directions (CPD) on admissibility of expert evidence in England and Wales were revised. They emphasised the principle that “the court must be satisfied that there is a sufficiently reliable scientific basis for the evidence to be admitted”. The present paper aims to assist courts in understanding from a scientific perspective what would be necessary to demonstrate the validity of testimony based on forensic voice comparison. We describe different technical approaches to forensic voice comparison that have been used in the UK, and critically review the case law on their admissibility. We conclude that courts have been inconsistent in their reasoning. In line with the CPD, we recommend that courts enquire as to whether forensic practitioners have made use of data and analytical methods that are appropriate and adequate for the case under consideration, and that courts require forensic practitioners to empirically demonstrate the level of performance of their forensic voice comparison system under conditions reflecting those of the case under consideration
Perception of natural vowels by monolingual Canadian-English, Mexican-Spanish, and Peninsular-Spanish listeners
On the basis of a previously-reported synthetic-vowel perception experiment, it was hypothesized that the location of the perceptual boundary between Spanish /i/ and /e/ differed for monolingual Peninsular-Spanish and Mexican-Spanish listeners (north-central Spain and Mexico City), and that this would affect the perception of the Canadian-English /i/-/i/ contrast (western Canada): Peninsular-Spanish listeners were predicted to identify almost all tokens of Canadian-English /i/ as Spanish /i/ and almost all tokens of Canadian-English /I/ as Spanish /e/ (two-category assimilation); whereas Mexican-Spanish listeners were predicted to identify almost all tokens of Canadian-English /i/ as Spanish /i/, but identify some tokens of Canadian-English /I/I as Spanish /i/ and some as Spanish /e/. Monolingual Peninsular-Spanish and Mexican-Spanish listeners' perception of natural tokens of English /i/, /I/, /e/, and /ε/ produced by monolingual Canadian-English speakers was tested. Both the Peninsular-Spanish and the Mexican-Spanish listeners had results consistent with the perceptual pattern predicted for the Peninsular-Spanish listeners. The results call into question the assumption that first-language-Spanish learners of English have difficulty learning the English /i/-/I/ contrast because they initially assimilate most tokens of both English vowel categories to a single Spanish vowel category, Spanish /i/
Ephedrine requirements are reduced during spinal anaesthesia for caesarean section in preeclampsia
Part of the Portfolio Thesis by Geoffrey H. Sharwood-Smith: The inferior vena caval compression theory of hypotension in obstetric spinal anaesthesia: studies in normal and preeclamptic pregnancy, a literature review and revision of fundamental concepts, available at http://hdl.handle.net/10023/1815Background:
Despite controversy over the haemodynamically safest blockade for caesarean section in women with severe preeclampsia, an increasing number of anaesthetists now opt for spinal anaesthesia. In a previous study we found that spinal compared to epidural anaesthesia offered an equally safe but more effective option for these patients. The current study was designed to compare the hypotension induced by spinal anaesthesia, as measured by ephedrine requirement, between 20 normotensive and 20 severely preeclamptic but haemodynamically stabilised women.
Method:
Standardised spinal anaesthesia was instituted and ephedrine was given in boluses of 6 mg if the systolic pressure fell >20% from the baseline, or if the patient exhibited symptoms of hypotension.
Results:
The mean ephedrine requirement of the normotensive group (27.9 ± 11.6 mg) was significantly greater (P < 0.01) than that of the preeclamptic group (16.4 ± 15.0 mg).
Conclusion:
This suggests that the hypotension induced by spinal anaesthesia in women with severe but haemodynamically stabilised preeclampsia, is less than that of normotensive patients.Publisher PD
- …