45 research outputs found

    A review of work done on Weber's law as applied to the intensity of sound

    Get PDF
    The following summary recapitulates the main points put forward in my review of Weber's law applied to the intensity of sound and the accompanying discussion. The topics dealt with are numbered to correspond with the division into sections. Certain general conclusions, not previously made, are also included here.I. Weber's law, or the Weber-Fechner law, if the name is to survive at all, must be interpreted on a psychological basis. That is to say, it must deal in some way with events of consciousness, since Weber's original statement dealt with the act of comparison, and Fechner's interest was in the relation between objective and subjective phenomena. Johnson's objection (152) that Fechner's formulation involved an undefined variable (sensation) is, on this view, invalid, since common experiences recognizes the real occurrence of sensation: as Boring (77) puts it, 'Everybody known what γ ( Fechner's S) is, independently of ß ( Fechner's R)', whereas what (ß itself is cannot be said with certainty. It is therefore more logical to define stimulus in terms of sensation than sensation in terms of stimulus. A psycho - physical interpretation must further be ruled out, since practically no -one now believes (though Kümmel (164) is a comparatively recent exception) that every smallest alteration of stimulus produces a sensation change.The possibility of a logarithmic relation between S and R, in whatever sense they may be interpreted, stands or falls not so much by the equality of just noticeable differences, but by the measurability of sensation. Although sensation is certainly not measurable in any sort of objective units, the general opinion seems now to be that in some sense or other sensation is measurable. This seems to have been the conclusion drawn at the Leicester meeting of the British Association (1932). In the symposium already quoted, Houston (149) stated that he personally was certain that sensation could be measured. The view that sensory discrimination is based on the occurrence or absence of awareness of sameness or difference, and that judgements of sameness or difference may be treated by the Law of Error was represented at that symposium by Bartlett (60). A synthesis of this view with a recognition of the effects of absolute 'bigness' and 'littleness' characterizing sensory events, would seem to afford a pragmatic justification of the measurement of sensation. The results of such measurement, if subjected to mathematical analysis, may or may not be found to take the form of a logarithmic equation.II. In the investigation of sensibility to intensity differences of sound many difficulties due to the physical properties of sound waves, and to peculiarities of the hearing mechanism, are encountered. Divergent results may be due to a variety of factors very hard to control; Sivian and White (229) call attention in particular to the possibility of effects of wave motion in the meatus, and of diffraction caused by the listener's head.The best results are probably to be obtained by using some apparatus which converts acoustical energy into equivalent units of electric current. In any case it, is advisable to use the decibel scale, since the response of the ear is at all events approximately logarithmic, so that simple units of pressure or energy flow soon reach unmanageable proportions.III. Certain other psychological properties of sound must also be considered, as well as certain phenomena which may be said to be of an intermediate nature. In dealing with any sound other than a pure tone, masking phenomena must be recognized, and compensated for if absolute measurement by summation is required. These phenomena, as Wegel and Lane (272) point out, are largely responsible for the non-occurrence of a linear relation at high intensities between sound pressure and response of the ear.In addition, sensory discrimination is affected by a large number of purely subjective factors, such as those listed by Fernberger (114) as governing equality judgements, e.g., attention, instructions, comprehension, temperament, etc. Some of the phenomena of hearing seem to have withstood all attempts at explanation in physical terms; on the other hand, one cannot agree with Watt(269) , who stated that all sensory experiences can be accounted for in systematic terms without recourse to the discoveries of physics or physiology.IV. A summary of work on auditory intensive thresholds has already been given (pp. 152-4). At best, Δ R/R for sound intensity is constant only for a limited range.V. Direct estimation or judgement of loudness in absolute units provides rather inconclusive results. Constancy is usually obtained within the bounds of an individual investigation, even among the different subjects participating in that investigation. On the other hand, agreement between investigators is rather exceptional, except in very general terms. Analysis of results, however, seems now to favour an exponential equation relating loudness to intensity of sound measured in db. The decibel scale, though useful in its own way is very misleading if interpreted as giving numerical indications of loudness.Measurement may also be accomplished by a method of balancing loudness, preferably against a definite reference tone. This is most difficult at the extremes of the frequency range, where the number of distinguishable intensities is small, and consequently, as Tucker (257) points out, it may not be possible to match with the reference tone.VI. Many writers have reduced the status of Weber's law to that of a special case of a generalized relativity law. Various attempts have also been made to determine a more accurate mathematical formulation representing sensory discrimination in the different fields.On the whole it is best to take Weber's law from the phenomenological point of view - i.e., as an approximation best describing the general form of a number of observed facts. Thus, we may, for example follow Lloyd Morgan (181), who proposed the modification: 'For constant increments of sensation, the concomitant increments of stimulus are in geometrical progression', introducing the qualification 'approximately'. Better still, it is possible to eliminate intensity from the problem altogether, and consider Weber's law as expressing a relation between different modes of consciousness (cf. Gatti (127), Cobb (98) ).The problem has also been approached from a physiological angle; In this cases sound intensity discrimination is bound up with the theory of hearing. Since it is not possible to work with living human ears, experimental work has been confined to work with animals, and with models of an 'artificial ear', (e.g. by Békésy (63) ), and Langenheck (167) who showed the limitations imposed upon this method by the use of imperfect materials.), The results of recent work seem to indicate that some theory based on the all-or-none principle must hold good, but work on the different sense-departments still awaits synthesis. The recent discoveries alluded to have rendered many standard and semi-popular books (e.g. that of Ogden (196)) out-of-date, and a complete survey of recent work on hearing would no doubt prove valuable.VII. The practical implications of the difference threshold are numerous, and in many cases too obvious to attract attention. A case in point is comprehension of speech, discussed by Marx (30).VIII. The original experiments here described lead to the following conclusions: (i) Weber's law holds very approximately for a limited range of sound intensities. (ii) The deviations are of a continuous nature. This bears out the findings of Kenneth and Thouless (23), and Riesz (36). Upper deviations seem to have been conclusively demonstrated. (iii) Individual differences are very noticeable, and often even surprising (cf. Weiss (274) ) , and day-to-day variations for the same observer may also occur. (iv) All the subjects tended, to quote a phrase used by Banister (56), to objectify their experience to a high degree. Thus in the watch-tick experiment, the criterion was often intuited nearness rather than apparent loudness. In the phonometer experiment, again, one subject (B) found it helpful to think of the sounds as produced by hammer -blows of varying force; another subject (C) equated the tuning -fork tones with the energy he would have required to sing them.(v) Different statistical procedures yield widely different thresholds. It is doubtful whether the labour of the Constant method ever justifies results. Interpolation, as advocated by Newhall (191, 192) is probably sufficient for most purposes. Thresholds based on Spearman's (295) or Wirth's (see 258)formulae seem to yield too low values

    Genetic variation I nnatural populations of field voles microtus agrestis (L)

    Get PDF
    That genetic mechanisms involving balanced selection pressures play a part in population dynamics has been advanced by Chitty (1960, 1965). He suggests that, as the population size increases, a "high density" genotype becomes selected, well adapted to the social stresses, which high density engenders. This genotype is, however, less well adapted to the normal selective pressures imposed by adverse environmental conditions. Severe weather conditions do not alone cause a population to crash. (Chitty 1957). However, according to Chitty's theory the higher the population density, and with it the greater the prevalence of high density genotype, the less severe need be the winter, in order to precipitate a crash.It is probable that the "high density" genotype will involve a large number of loci, each making some contribution to the ability of the animal to survive in crowded conditions. Since the theory also calls for the high density genotype to be less well suited to adverse environmental conditions, these will tend to select out those alleles responsible for the high density genotype. Thus, in fact, a system of balanced selection will exist, and the loci involved will be maintained in a polymorphic condition.Although it is unlikely that the Es -1 locus plays a major part in controlling the population processes, it is interesting to speculate on the possibility. In such a case the E₁ negative animals constitute Chitty's high density genotype since they show an increase in frequency when the population is high. That E₁- negative animals are less well adapted than E₁-positive animals to winter conditions, it is clear from the results of the preceding chapter.It may be shown that Chitty's theory appears equally plausible expressed in reverse. Rapidly increasing populations are generally exposed to fairly limited selective pressures. For instance, in the first study area described in the previous chapter, no systematic changes in phenotypic frequencies were observed during the initial stages of the population increase before the crash. It is probable that the normal selective pressures are those operative during normal winters. If this is so, then each year, as the optimum phenotype becomes more common, winter mortality would be reduced and the population present at the start of each successive breeding season would increase, with a corresponding great increase in the population level attained towards the end of the breeding season. Presumably a year would be reached when social stress would exert strong selective pressure. The population size would be reduced, during which time the animals best adapted to stress would tend to survive. If, in fact, these animals were susceptible to severe climatic conditions, the winter months would tend to reduce the population still further. In the spring the population density would therefore be much lower than normal; the population would have "crashed."Chitty (1965) suggests methods by means of which his theory might be checked. These would consist of setting up a series of artificial populations of the same size, using either animals from an expanding population (high density genotypes) or a declining one. His theory would then predict that the first population should continue to expand, while the second one would not. Further populations of the first type should prove less well adapted to adverse weather conditions. These predictions are reversed in the case of the reversed form of Chitty's theory. According to it, an increasing population is one in which gradual adaptation to winter conditions has taken place, while the declining populations are the survivors of a density dependent selection process, and are therefore likely to be well adapted to high population density.If suitable parameter for selective pressures and population growth rate could be estimated from field studies, it ought to be possible to produce a computer programme to simulate the natural population processes

    A serum esterase variation in Microtus agrestis

    Full text link
    corecore