574 research outputs found
Computational Agent-based Models in Opinion Dynamics: A Survey on Social Simulations and Empirical Studies
Understanding how an individual changes its attitude, belief, and opinion due
to other people's social influences is vital because of its wide implications.
A core methodology that is used to study the change of attitude under social
influences is agent-based model (ABM). The goal of this review paper is to
compare and contrast existing ABMs, which I classify into two families, the
deductive ABMs and the inductive ABMs. The former subsumes social simulation
studies, and the latter involves human experiments. To facilitate the
comparison between ABMs of different formulations, I propose a general unified
formulation, in which all ABMs can be viewed as special cases. In addition, I
show the connections between deductive ABMs and inductive ABMs, and point out
their strengths and limitations. At the end of the paper, I identify
underexplored areas and suggest future research directions.Comment: 57 page
Human-machine cooperation for semantic feature listing
Semantic feature norms, lists of features that concepts do and do not
possess, have played a central role in characterizing human conceptual
knowledge, but require extensive human labor. Large language models (LLMs)
offer a novel avenue for the automatic generation of such feature lists, but
are prone to significant error. Here, we present a new method for combining a
learned model of human lexical-semantics from limited data with LLM-generated
data to efficiently generate high-quality feature norms.Comment: To be published in the ICLR TinyPaper trac
Reverse-engineering the cortical architecture for controlled semantic cognition.
We employ a reverse-engineering approach to illuminate the neurocomputational building blocks that combine to support controlled semantic cognition: the storage and context-appropriate use of conceptual knowledge. By systematically varying the structure of a computational model and assessing the functional consequences, we identified the architectural properties that best promote some core functions of the semantic system. Semantic cognition presents a challenging test case, as the brain must achieve two seemingly contradictory functions: abstracting context-invariant conceptual representations across time and modalities, while producing specific context-sensitive behaviours appropriate for the immediate task. These functions were best achieved in models possessing a single, deep multimodal hub with sparse connections from modality-specific regions, and control systems acting on peripheral rather than deep network layers. The reverse-engineered model provides a unifying account of core findings in the cognitive neuroscience of controlled semantic cognition, including evidence from anatomy, neuropsychology and functional brain imaging
"Pre-semantic" cognition revisited: Critical differences between semantic aphasia and semantic dementia
Patients with semantic dementia show a specific pattern of impairment on both verbal and non-verbal “pre-semantic” tasks: e.g., reading aloud, past tense generation, spelling to dictation, lexical decision, object decision, colour decision and delayed picture copying. All seven tasks are characterised by poorer performance for items that are atypical of the domain and “regularisation errors” (irregular/atypical items are produced as if they were domain-typical). The emergence of this pattern across diverse tasks in the same patients indicates that semantic memory plays a key role in all of these types of “pre-semantic” processing. However, this claim remains controversial because semantically-impaired patients sometimes fail to show an influence of regularity. This study demonstrates that (a) the location of brain damage and (b) the underlying nature of the semantic deficit affect the likelihood of observing the expected relationship between poor comprehension and regularity effects. We compared the effect of multimodal semantic impairment in the context of semantic dementia and stroke aphasia on the seven “pre-semantic” tasks listed above. In all of these tasks, the semantic aphasia patients were less sensitive to typicality than the semantic dementia patients, even though the two groups obtained comparable scores on semantic tests. The semantic aphasia group also made fewer regularisation errors and many more unrelated and perseverative responses. We propose that these group differences reflect the different locus for the semantic impairment in the two conditions: patients with semantic dementia have degraded semantic representations, whereas semantic aphasia patients show deregulated semantic cognition with concomitant executive deficits. These findings suggest a reinterpretation of single case studies of comprehension-impaired aphasic patients who fail to show the expected effect of regularity on “pre-semantic” tasks. Consequently, such cases do not demonstrate the independence of these tasks from semantic memory
Semantic memory is impaired in patients with unilateral anterior temporal lobe resection for temporal lobe epilepsy
Contemporary clinical and basic neuroscience studies have increasingly implicated the anterior temporal lobe regions, bilaterally, in the formation of coherent concepts. Mounting convergent evidence for the importance of the anterior temporal lobe in semantic memory is found in patients with bilateral anterior temporal lobe damage (e.g. semantic dementia), functional neuroimaging and repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation studies. If this proposal is correct, then one might expect patients with anterior temporal lobe resection for long-standing temporal lobe epilepsy to be semantically impaired. Such patients, however, do not present clinically with striking comprehension deficits but with amnesia and variable anomia, leading some to conclude that semantic memory is intact in resection for temporal lobe epilepsy and thus casting doubt over the conclusions drawn from semantic dementia and linked basic neuroscience studies. Whilst there is a considerable neuropsychological literature on temporal lobe epilepsy, few studies have probed semantic memory directly, with mixed results, and none have undertaken the same type of systematic investigation of semantic processing that has been conducted with other patient groups. In this study, therefore, we investigated the semantic performance of 20 patients with resection for chronic temporal lobe epilepsy with a full battery of semantic assessments, including more sensitive measures of semantic processing. The results provide a bridge between the current clinical observations about resection for temporal lobe epilepsy and the expectations from semantic dementia and other neuroscience findings. Specifically, we found that on simple semantic tasks, the patients’ accuracy fell in the normal range, with the exception that some patients with left resection for temporal lobe epilepsy had measurable anomia. Once the semantic assessments were made more challenging, by probing specific-level concepts, lower frequency/more abstract items or measuring reaction times on semantic tasks versus those on difficulty-matched non-semantic assessments, evidence of a semantic impairment was found in all individuals. We conclude by describing a unified, computationally inspired framework for capturing the variable degrees of semantic impairment found across different patient groups (semantic dementia, temporal lobe epilepsy, glioma and stroke) as well as semantic processing in neurologically intact participants
Fusiform activation to animals is driven by the process, not the stimulus
Previous studies have found that the lateral posterior fusiform gyri respond more robustly to pictures of animals than pictures of manmade objects and suggested that these regions encode the visual properties characteristic of animals. We suggest that such effects actually reflect processing demands arising when items with similar representations must be finely discriminated. In a positron emission tomography (PET) study of category verification with colored photographs of animals and vehicles, there was robust animal-specific activation in the lateral posterior fusiform gyri when stimuli were categorized at an intermediate level of specificity (e.g., dog or car). However, when the same photographs were categorized at a more specific level (e.g., Labrador or BMW), these regions responded equally strongly to animals and vehicles. We conclude that the lateral posterior fusiform does not encode domain-specific representations of animals or visual properties characteristic of animals. Instead, these regions are strongly activated whenever an item must be discriminated from many close visual or semantic competitors. Apparent category effects arise because, at an intermediate level of specificity, animals have more visual and semantic competitors than do artifacts
Learning interactions to boost human creativity with bandits and GPT-4
This paper considers how interactions with AI algorithms can boost human
creative thought. We employ a psychological task that demonstrates limits on
human creativity, namely semantic feature generation: given a concept name,
respondents must list as many of its features as possible. Human participants
typically produce only a fraction of the features they know before getting
"stuck." In experiments with humans and with a language AI (GPT-4) we contrast
behavior in the standard task versus a variant in which participants can ask
for algorithmically-generated hints. Algorithm choice is administered by a
multi-armed bandit whose reward indicates whether the hint helped generating
more features. Humans and the AI show similar benefits from hints, and
remarkably, bandits learning from AI responses prefer the same prompting
strategy as those learning from human behavior. The results suggest that
strategies for boosting human creativity via computer interactions can be
learned by bandits run on groups of simulated participants
- …