55 research outputs found
On Bohr-Sommerfeld bases
This paper combines algebraic and Lagrangian geometry to construct a special
basis in every space of conformal blocks, the Bohr-Sommerfeld (BS) basis. We
use the method of [D. Borthwick, T. Paul and A. Uribe, Legendrian distributions
with applications to the non-vanishing of Poincar\'e series of large weight,
Invent. math, 122 (1995), 359-402, preprint hep-th/9406036], whereby every
vector of a BS basis is defined by some half-weighted Legendrian distribution
coming from a Bohr-Sommerfeld fibre of a real polarization of the underlying
symplectic manifold. The advantage of BS bases (compared to bases of theta
functions in [A. Tyurin, Quantization and ``theta functions'', Jussieu preprint
216 (Apr 1999), e-print math.AG/9904046, 32pp.]) is that we can use information
from the skillful analysis of the asymptotics of quantum states. This gives
that Bohr-Sommerfeld bases are unitary quasi-classically. Thus we can apply
these bases to compare the Hitchin connection with the KZ connection defined by
the monodromy of the Knizhnik-Zamolodchikov equation in combinatorial theory
(see, for example, [T. Kohno, Topological invariants for 3-manifolds using
representations of mapping class group I, Topology 31 (1992), 203-230; II,
Contemp. math 175} (1994), 193-217]).Comment: 43 pages, uses: latex2e with amsmath,amsfonts,theore
Long Lasting Egocentric Disorientation Induced by Normal Sensori-Motor Spatial Interaction
Perception of the cardinal directions of the body, right-left, up-down, ahead-behind, which appears so absolute and fundamental to the organisation of behaviour can in fact, be modified. Perhaps unsurprisingly, it has been shown that prolonged distorted perception of the orientation of body axes can be a consequence of disordered sensori-motor signals, including long-term prismatic adaptation and lesions of the central nervous system. We report the novel and surprising finding that a long-lasting distortion of perception of personal space can also be induced by an ecological pointing task without the artifice of distorting normal sensori-motor relationships.Twelve right-handed healthy adults performed the task of pointing with their arms, without vision, to indicate their subjective 'straight ahead', a task often used to assess the Egocentric Reference. This was performed before, immediately, and one day after a second task intended to 'modulate' perception of spatial direction. The 'modulating' task lasted 5 minutes and consisted of asking participants to point with the right finger to targets that appeared only in one (right or left) half of a computer screen. Estimates of the 'straight-ahead' during pre-test were accurate (inferior to 0.3 degrees deviation). Significantly, up to one day after performing the modulating task, the subjective 'straight-ahead' was deviated (by approximately 3.2 degrees) to the same side to which subjects had pointed to targets.These results reveal that the perception of directional axes for behaviour is readily influenced by interactions with the environment that involve no artificial distortion of normal sensori-motor-spatial relationships and does not necessarily conform to the cardinal directions as defined by the anatomy of orthostatic posture. We thus suggest that perceived space is a dynamic construction directly dependent upon our past experience about the direction and/or the localisation of our sensori-motor spatial interaction with environment
The role of light scatter in the residual visual sensitivity of patients with complete cerebral hemispherectomy.
Various residual visual capacities have been reported for the phenomenally blind field of hemispherectomized patients, providing evidence for the relative roles of cortical and subcortical pathways in vision. We attempted to characterize these functions by examining the ability of five patients to detect, localize, and discriminate high-contrast flashed, flickering and moving targets. Dependent measures were verbal, manual, and oculomotor responses. As a control for light scatter, intensity thresholds for monocular detection of targets in the hemianopic field were compared with thresholds obtained when using an additional half eyepatch to occlude the blind hemiretina of the tested eye. One unilaterally destriate patient was tested on the same tasks. In photopic conditions, none of the hemispherectomized patients could respond to visual cues in their impaired fields, whereas the destriate patient could detect, discriminate, and point to targets, and appreciate the apparent motion of stimuli across his midline. Under reduced lighting, the threshold luminance required by hemispherectomized patients to detect stimuli presented monocularly was similar to that required for their detection when all visual information was occluded in the blind field, and only available to the visual system indirectly via light scatter. In contrast, the destriate patient's monocular threshold in his blind field was substantially lower than that for stimuli directly occluded in the blind field. As we found no range of stimuli which the hemispherectomized patients could detect or discriminate that was not also associated with discriminable scattered light, we conclude that the subcortical pathways which survive hemispherectomy cannot mediate voluntary behavioural responses to visual information in the hemianopic field
Hemifield-specific visual recognition memory impairments in patients with unilateral temporal lobe removals.
Recent evidence on visual neglect suggests that each hemisphere maintains a retinotopically organized representation of the visual world contralateral to the current fixation point and that this representation is based not only on analysis of the current retinal input but, equally importantly, on information retrieved from memory. This idea predicts that unilateral damage to memory systems should produce a lateralized impairment of memory for the retinotopically contralateral visual world. To test this prediction we examined visual recognition memory performance in the left and right visual hemifields of patients who had undergone partial unilateral temporal lobe removals for the relief of epilepsy, either in the left hemisphere (n = 5) or the right (n = 5). The patients were given complex artificial scenes to remember, constructed of independent left and right halves, and were then tested for recognition of the left and the right halves separately. Stimuli were exposed tachistoscopically throughout and fixation was maintained on a central position. Patients made significantly more errors with half-scenes in the hemifield contralateral to their removal than in the ipsilateral hemifield, an increase of 50% in the error rate on average. The effect was seen equally in patients with left and right removals. This finding supports the idea that visual memory retrieval is retinotopically organized
Trigonal curves and Galois Spin(8)-bundles
this paper we study the moduli of principal holomorphic Spin(8)-bundles over an algebraic curve. However, in order to exploit triality, and to obtain a moduli space with particularly nice properties, we impose some additional constraints on our bundles. We suppose that X is an algebraic curve with an S 3 -action. The group S 3 then acts in two ways on Spin(8)-bundles over a curve: by pull-back under the action on X, and by the triality action on the structure group. We call a Spin(8)-bundle Galois if these two actions coincide, that is, if it is a fixed point of the group actio
Quantitative fMRI assessment of the differences in lateralization of language-related brain activation in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy
Defining language lateralization is important to minimize morbidity in patients treated surgically for temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) offers a promising, noninvasive, alternative strategy to the Wada test. Here we have used fMRI to study healthy controls and patients with TLE in order to (i) define language-related activation patterns and their reproducibility; (ii) compare lateralization determined by fMRI with those from of the Wada test; and (iii) contrast different methods of assessing fMRI lateralization. Twelve healthy right-handed controls and 19 right-handed preoperative patients with TLE (12 left- and seven right-TLE) were studied at 3T using fMRI and a verbal fluency paradigm. A Wada test also was performed on each of the patients. Greater activation was found in several areas in the right hemisphere for the left-TLE group relative to controls or right-TLE patients. Relative hemispheric activations calculated based on either the extent or the mean signal change gave consistent results showing a more bihemispheric language representation in the left-TLE patients. There was good agreement between the Wada and fMRI results, although the latter were more sensitive to involvement of the nondominant right hemisphere. The reproducibility of the fMRI values was lowest for the more bihemispherically represented left-TLE patients. Overall, our results further demonstrate that noninvasive fMRI measures of language-related lateralization may provide a practical and reliable alternative to invasive testing for presurgical language lateralization in patients with TLE. The high proportion (33%) of left-TLE patients showing bilateral or right hemispheric language-related lateralization suggests that there is considerable plasticity of language representation in the brains of patients with intractable TLE
- …