173 research outputs found
Recommended from our members
Sound language policies must be consistent with natural language evolution
Societal multilingualism and multilectalism have been among the leading justifications for language policies, especially in the Global South, where many of these have failed. I associate the failures with poor choices of official languages and media of education, which are not consistent with the linguistic behaviors of the majority of the citizenry and the socioeconomic structures of the relevant polities. I review some cases of adequate and inadequate policies around the world and explain ecologically some reasons for either their successes or their failures. In a subset of the cases, I assess the results as mixed. My recommendation is of course not to follow the policy of a particular polity simply because it has succeeded there but to also check whether the ecology of its success is similar to that of the new polity. The relevant ecology includes the socioeconomic structure/system and the linguistic practices of the citizenry for whom the policy is intended. Among the issues to address is, for instance, whether the language adopted as the official language and medium of education is easy for the majority of the citizenry to learn successfully. Another is whether the language policy will make the economic development of the nation more inclusive and empower the majority economically and politically
Modeling the emergence of contact languages
Contact languages are born out of the non-trivial interaction of two (or more) parent languages.
Nowadays, the enhanced possibility of mobility and communication allows for a
strong mixing of languages and cultures, thus raising the issue of whether there are any
pure languages or cultures that are unaffected by contact with others. As with bacteria or viruses
in biological evolution, the evolution of languages is marked by horizontal transmission;
but to date no reliable quantitative tools to investigate these phenomena have been
available. An interesting and well documented example of contact language is the emergence
of creole languages, which originated in the contacts of European colonists and
slaves during the 17th and 18th centuries in exogenous plantation colonies of especially the
Atlantic and Indian Ocean. Here, we focus on the emergence of creole languages to demonstrate
a dynamical process that mimics the process of creole formation in American and
Caribbean plantation ecologies. Inspired by the Naming Game (NG), our modeling scheme
incorporates demographic information about the colonial population in the framework of a
non-trivial interaction network including three populations: Europeans, Mulattos/Creoles,
and Bozal slaves. We show how this sole information makes it possible to discriminate territories
that produced modern creoles from those that did not, with a surprising accuracy. The
generality of our approach provides valuable insights for further studies on the emergence
of languages in contact ecologies as well as to test specific hypotheses about the peopling
and the population structures of the relevant territories. We submit that these tools could be
relevant to addressing problems related to contact phenomena in many cultural domains:
e.g., emergence of dialects, language competition and hybridization,
globalization phenomena
Restructuring, Feature Selection, and Markedness: From Kimanyanga to Kituba
Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics
Society: Special Session on Historical Issues in African Linguistics
(1994
Les créoles. L'état de notre savoir
RÉSUMÉLes créoles. L'état de notre savoirDans cet article, je définis les vernaculaires créoles d'un point de vue sociohistorique plutôt que structurel et je passe en revue la littérature qui explique comment ils se sont développés. Pendant cet exercice, j'explicite des problèmes qui émanent des définitions traditionnelles et ce dont les hypothèses courantes de genèse ne rendent pas compte et j'argue que la meilleure approche est la linguistique historique et génétique. Celle-ci est fondée dans les histoires socioéconomiques où se sont développés ces vernaculaires. elle reconnaît les différents dialectes des langues lexificatrices qui sont entrées en contact avec des langues non européennes et elle articule les principes qui régissent la sélection des traits intégrés dans le vernaculaire naissant à partir des variétés en compétition.Mots clefs : Mufwene. créole, substrat, superstrat. lexificatrice. relexificationABSTRACTCréoles : The State of the AnIn this article I define creole vernaculars sociohistorically. rather than strucrurally. and survey the literature that accounts for how they developed. In this exercise I explain the problems inherent to traditional definitions and what is not accounted for by current hypotheses of creole genesis. I argue that the best approach to theeeee subject matter is one that treats the development of creoles as a topic of historical and genetic linguistics. well grounded in the socio-economie histories in which these vernaculars developed. factoring in the relevant dialects of the lexifiers that came in contact with non-European languages. and articulating principles that govern the sélection of features from the competing varieties into the emerging vemacular.Key words: Mufwene. creole, substratum. superstratum. lexifier. relexificatio
Recommended from our members
Modeling the Emergence of Contact Languages
Contact languages are born out of the non-trivial interaction of two (or more) parent languages. Nowadays, the enhanced possibility of mobility and communication allows for a strong mixing of languages and cultures, thus raising the issue of whether there are any pure languages or cultures that are unaffected by contact with others. As with bacteria or viruses in biological evolution, the evolution of languages is marked by horizontal transmission; but to date no reliable quantitative tools to investigate these phenomena have been available. An interesting and well documented example of contact language is the emergence of creole languages, which originated in the contacts of European colonists and slaves during the 17th and 18th centuries in exogenous plantation colonies of especially the Atlantic and Indian Ocean. Here, we focus on the emergence of creole languages to demonstrate a dynamical process that mimics the process of creole formation in American and Caribbean plantation ecologies. Inspired by the Naming Game (NG), our modeling scheme incorporates demographic information about the colonial population in the framework of a non-trivial interaction network including three populations: Europeans, Mulattos/Creoles, and Bozal slaves. We show how this sole information makes it possible to discriminate territories that produced modern creoles from those that did not, with a surprising accuracy. The generality of our approach provides valuable insights for further studies on the emergence of languages in contact ecologies as well as to test specific hypotheses about the peopling and the population structures of the relevant territories. We submit that these tools could be relevant to addressing problems related to contact phenomena in many cultural domains: e.g., emergence of dialects, language competition and hybridization, globalization phenomena.</p
Utterance Selection Model of Language Change
We present a mathematical formulation of a theory of language change. The
theory is evolutionary in nature and has close analogies with theories of
population genetics. The mathematical structure we construct similarly has
correspondences with the Fisher-Wright model of population genetics, but there
are significant differences. The continuous time formulation of the model is
expressed in terms of a Fokker-Planck equation. This equation is exactly
soluble in the case of a single speaker and can be investigated analytically in
the case of multiple speakers who communicate equally with all other speakers
and give their utterances equal weight. Whilst the stationary properties of
this system have much in common with the single-speaker case, time-dependent
properties are richer. In the particular case where linguistic forms can become
extinct, we find that the presence of many speakers causes a two-stage
relaxation, the first being a common marginal distribution that persists for a
long time as a consequence of ultimate extinction being due to rare
fluctuations.Comment: 21 pages, 17 figure
Predicting language diversity with complex network
Evolution and propagation of the world's languages is a complex phenomenon,
driven, to a large extent, by social interactions. Multilingual society can be
seen as a system of interacting agents, where the interaction leads to a
modification of the language spoken by the individuals. Two people can reach
the state of full linguistic compatibility due to the positive interactions,
like transfer of loanwords. But, on the other hand, if they speak entirely
different languages, they will separate from each other. These simple
observations make the network science the most suitable framework to describe
and analyze dynamics of language change. Although many mechanisms have been
explained, we lack a qualitative description of the scaling behavior for
different sizes of a population. Here we address the issue of the language
diversity in societies of different sizes, and we show that local interactions
are crucial to capture characteristics of the empirical data. We propose a
model of social interactions, extending the idea from, that explains the growth
of the language diversity with the size of a population of country or society.
We argue that high clustering and network disintegration are the most important
characteristics of models properly describing empirical data. Furthermore, we
cancel the contradiction between previous models and the Solomon Islands case.
Our results demonstrate the importance of the topology of the network, and the
rewiring mechanism in the process of language change
Aging in language dynamics
Human languages evolve continuously, and a puzzling problem is how to reconcile the apparent robustness of most of the deep linguistic structures we use with the evidence that they undergo possibly slow, yet ceaseless, changes. Is the state in which we observe languages today closer to what would be a dynamical attractor with statistically stationary properties or rather closer to a non-steady state slowly evolving in time? Here we address this question in the framework of the emergence of shared linguistic categories in a population of individuals interacting through language games. The observed emerging asymptotic categorization, which has been previously tested - with success - against experimental data from human languages, corresponds to a metastable state where global shifts are always possible but progressively more unlikely and the response properties depend on the age of the system. This aging mechanism exhibits striking quantitative analogies to what is observed in the statistical mechanics of glassy systems. We argue that this can be a general scenario in language dynamics where shared linguistic conventions would not emerge as attractors, but rather as metastable states
- …