2,118 research outputs found
Public statistics and private experience: varying feedback information in a take or pass game
We study how subjects in an experiment use different forms of public
information about their opponentsâ past behaviour. In the absence
of public information, subjects appear to use rather detailed statistics
summarizing their private experiences. If they have additional public
information, they make use of this information even if it is less precise
than their own private statisticsâexcept for very high stakes. Making
public information more precise has two consequences: It is also
used when the stakes are very high and it reduces the number of subjects
who ignore any informationâpublic and private. That is, precise
public information crowds in the use of own information. Finally, our
results shed some light on unravelling in centipede games
Endogenous leadership in teams
In this paper we study the mechanics of âleading by exampleâ in
teams. Leadership is beneficial for the entire team when agents are
conformists, i.e., dislike effort differentials. We also show how leadership
can arise endogenously and discuss what type of leader benefits
a team most
Learning spillover and analogy-based expectations: a multi-game experiment
We consider a multi-game interactive learning environment and ask ourselves
whether long run behaviors in one game are a€ected by behaviors in the other,
i.e whether there are learning spillovers. Our main ïżœnding is that learning
spillovers arise whenever the feedback provided to subjects about past play is
not easily accessible game by game and thus subjects get a more immediate
impression about aggregate distributions. In such a case, long run behaviors
stabilize to an analogy-based expectation equilibrium (Jehiel 2005), thereby
suggesting how one should broaden the notion of equilibrium to cope with
learning spillovers
Magic numbers in the discrete tomography of cyclotomic model sets
We report recent progress in the problem of distinguishing convex subsets of
cyclotomic model sets by (discrete parallel) X-rays in prescribed
-directions. It turns out that for any of these model sets
there exists a `magic number' such that any two
convex subsets of can be distinguished by their X-rays in any set
of prescribed -directions. In particular, for
pentagonal, octagonal, decagonal and dodecagonal model sets, the least possible
numbers are in that very order 11, 9, 11 and 13.Comment: 6 pages, 1 figure; based on the results of arXiv:1101.4149 [math.MG];
presented at Aperiodic 2012 (Cairns, Australia
Niveaux d'énergie de 28Si au voisinage de 13,245 MeV observés à l'aide des réactions 27Al(p, γ)28Si et 27Al(p, α) 24Mg
L'Ă©tude simultanĂ©e des caractĂ©ristiques des rĂ©sonances de la rĂ©action 27Al + p Ă une Ă©nergie Ep = 1 724 keV, pour diffĂ©rentes voies de sortie, a permis de conclure Ă l'existence de deux niveaux distincts (JÏ = 3-, JÏ = 5-) dans 28Si situĂ©s Ă 13 245 ± 2 keV
Simulation-based Testing for Early Safety-Validation of Robot Systems
Industrial human-robot collaborative systems must be validated thoroughly
with regard to safety. The sooner potential hazards for workers can be exposed,
the less costly is the implementation of necessary changes. Due to the
complexity of robot systems, safety flaws often stay hidden, especially at
early design stages, when a physical implementation is not yet available for
testing. Simulation-based testing is a possible way to identify hazards in an
early stage. However, creating simulation conditions in which hazards become
observable can be difficult. Brute-force or Monte-Carlo-approaches are often
not viable for hazard identification, due to large search spaces. This work
addresses this problem by using a human model and an optimization algorithm to
generate high-risk human behavior in simulation, thereby exposing potential
hazards. A proof of concept is shown in an application example where the method
is used to find hazards in an industrial robot cell
Relationship between moonlight and nightly activity patterns of the ocelot (Leopardus pardalis) and some of its prey species in Formosa, Northern Argentina
The moon can profoundly influence the activity patterns of animals. If predators are more successful under bright moonlight, prey species are likely to respond by shifting their own activity patterns (predator-avoidance hypothesis). However, the assumption that prey will necessarily avoid full-moon nights does not take into account that moonlight also allows prey to more easily detect predators, and to forage more efficiently. Thus, nightly activity patterns could depend on night vision capabilities (visual-acuity hypothesis). To consider the possible influences of moonlight and to distinguish between these hypotheses, we used camera-trapping records of a predator, the ocelot (Leopardus pardalis), and several of its night-active prey to compare activity patterns under different moonlight conditions. The ocelots' activity patterns were not strongly related to moonlight, but showed a slight tendency for higher activity during brighter nights. Tapeti rabbits (Sylvilagus brasiliensis) and brocket deer (Mazama americana) showed a clear preference for brighter nights. White-eared opossums (Didelphis albiventris) also showed a trend to be less active in new moon light. In contrast, smaller grey four-eyed opossums (Philander opossum) and the poor eye-sight nine-banded armadillo (Dasypus novemcinctus) showed similar activity patterns across all moon phases. Since activity patterns of most prey species were not shifted away from the activity of the ocelot, the differences between species are probably linked to their night vision capabilities, and emphasise the need for more information on the visual system of these taxa. Their activity patterns seem to be less strongly linked to avoidance of predation than previously thought, suggesting that foraging and predator detection benefits may play a more important role than usually acknowledged.The cameras were partly funded by a National Geographic Society/Waitt grant to
MH (grant number NGS 1072-78) and partly by quality-related funding through the Research
Excellence Framework to the Biological Sciences Research Group of the University of Derby.
The long-term camera-trap monitoring was in part made possible through grants to EFD and the Owl Monkey Project from the L.S.B. Leakey Foundation, National Geographic Society and the National Science Foundation of the USA (NSF-BCS-0621020, 0837921 (REU), 0924352 (REU), 1026991 (REU), 1219368 (RAPID) and 1232349 (2012)
- âŠ