14 research outputs found
LLMs don't know anything: reply to Yildirim and Paul
In their recent Opinion in TiCS, Yildirim and Paul propose that large language models (LLMs) have âinstrumental knowledgeâ and possibly the kind of âworldlyâ knowledge that humans do. They suggest that the production of appropriate outputs by LLMs is evidence that LLMs infer âtask structureâ that may reflect âcausal abstractions of... entities and processes in the real world.' While we agree that LLMs are impressive and potentially interesting for cognitive science, we resist this project on two grounds. First, it casts LLMs as agents rather than as models. Second, it suggests that causal understanding could be acquired from the capacity for mere prediction
Recommended from our members
Chimpanzees prepare for alternative possible outcomes
When facing uncertainty, humans often build mental models of alternative outcomes. Considering diverging scenarios allows agents to respond adaptively to different actual worlds by developing contingency plans (covering one's bases). In a pre-registered experiment, we tested whether chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) prepare for two mutually exclusive possibilities. Chimpanzees could access two pieces of food, but only if they successfully protected them from a human competitor. In one condition, chimpanzees could be certain about which piece of food the human experimenter would attempt to steal. In a second condition, either one of the food rewards was a potential target of the competitor. We found that chimpanzees were significantly more likely to protect both pieces of food in the second relative to the first condition, raising the possibility that chimpanzees represent and prepare effectively for different possible worlds.PostprintPeer reviewe
Recommended from our members
Certain to be surprised:A preference for novel causal outcomes develops in early childhood
A large literature on the development of causal reasoningcharacterizes early childhood as a period of curiosity,exploration, and experimentation. This suggests that a noveltypreference may be a universal hallmark of early causallearning. Functionally, such a bias might serve to directattention towards new opportunities for knowledge gain. Analternative possibility is that a preference for exploring noveloutcomes develops over time. In three experiments with 2- to5-year-olds, we investigate the developmental trajectory ofchildrenâs preference for causal processes that producereliable versus novel outcomes. We find evidence for adevelopmental shift between ages 2 and 3: while two-year-olds trend toward a preference for reliable over noveloutcomes, older children clearly prefer novel ones. Wediscuss possible adaptive reasons for this developmental shift
Recommended from our members
Toddlers and Adults Simultaneously Track Multiple Hypotheses in a CausalLearning Task
Research on the development of future hypothetical andcounterfactual thinking suggests that children as old as fivemay be unable to consider multiple, equally probablepossibilities simultaneously. Yet, a large literature on thedevelopment of causal reasoning suggests that much youngerchildren are able to generate, evaluate, and test causalhypotheses, often by integrating information about severalcandidate causes at once. The current research seeks to bridgethese two bodies of research. In three experiments, adults andtoddlers (18â30 months) observe a sequence of evidence thatis equally consistent with two hypotheses, each occupying adifferent level of abstraction (individual vs. relational).Results suggest that learners generate more than one potentialcause, hold both in mind, and flexibly apply the appropriatehypothesis to inform their inferences at test. Findingschallenge previous suggestions that much older children failto consider multiple, equally probable possibilities
Recommended from our members
Providing explanations shifts preschoolersâ metaphor preferences
In order to learn from metaphors, children must not only be able to understand metaphors, but also appreciate their relative informativeness. Although functional metaphors based on abstract commonalities (e.g. âEyes are windowsâ) allow for more learning than perceptual metaphors based on superficial commonalities (e.g. âEyes are buttonsâ), previous research shows that preschoolers prefer perceptual metaphors over functional metaphors. In the present studies, we ask whether providing additional context can shift metaphor preferences in preschoolers and adults. Experiment 1 finds that pedagogical context increases preferences for functional metaphors in adults, but not preschoolers. Experiment 2 finds that providing explanations for conceptual similarities in a metaphor increases preschoolersâ preferences for functional metaphors. These findings suggest that providing explanations allows even preschoolers to appreciate the informativeness of functional metaphors
Recommended from our members
4- and 5-Year-Oldsâ Comprehension of Functional Metaphors
Previous work suggests that childrenâs ability to understand
metaphors emerges late in development. Researchers argue that
childrenâs initial failure to understand metaphors is due to an
inability to reason about shared relational structures between
concepts. However, recent work demonstrates that causal framing
facilitates preschoolersâ relational reasoning. Might causal framing
also facilitate preschoolersâ metaphor comprehension? In
Experiment 1, we presented 128 4- to 5-year-olds with a novel
metaphor comprehension task, following a causal warm-up task,
control warm-up task, or no warm-up task. In the novel
comprehension task, preschoolers rated functional metaphors and
nonsense statements as smart or silly, and provided explanations.
Preschoolers ranked metaphors as âsmarterâ than nonsense
statements, and a quarter of preschoolers provided functional
explanations. There was no effect of warm-up tasks. In Experiment
2, we validated the metaphor comprehension task with adults.
Overall, the current work presents a new paradigm that
demonstrates preschoolersâ capacity to understand functional
metaphors
Recommended from our members
Reasoning from Samples to Populations: Children Use Variability Information to Predict Novel Outcomes
The ability to infer general characteristics of populations from specific instances is critical for reasoning. While there is evidence of this capacity in infancy, prior work has not examined childrenâs ability to use these second-order inferences to make predictions about future outcomes. In the current study, 3-year-olds observed balls drawn at random from two containers. In one sample each ball was a different color. The other sample consisted of balls of only one (Experiment 1) or two (Experiment 2) colors. Children were asked which of the containers was more likely to contain a novel colored ball. A significant majority of children chose the more variable sampleâs container. This suggests that 3-year-olds are not only able to make inferences about hidden populations from the variability of observed samples, but also use those inferences to reason beyond their direct experience
Recommended from our members
What else could happen? Two-, three-, and four-year-olds use variabilityinformation to infer novel causal outcomes
Young children rapidly infer causal relations by trackingcontingencies between causes and their effects, and cangeneralize these rules to novel instances of the same cause.However, this is distinct from the ability to make inferencesabout whether a particular cause is likely to produce noveleffects. Here, we investigate the development of two-, three-,and four-year-oldsâ ability to recognize and use informationabout a causeâs variability to make predictions about othernovel outcomes it might produce. Experiment 1 finds thatchildren as young as two years of age infer that a cause thathas produced variable, rather than deterministic outcomes ismore likely to produce a novel, previously unobserved effect.Experiment 2 finds that four-year-olds, but not two- andthree-year-olds, infer that a higher variability cause is morelikely to produce a novel outcome than a lower variabilitycause
Recommended from our members
Enhancing Effects of Causal Scaffolding on Preschoolers' Analogical Reasoning Abilities
Decades of work exploring the development of childrenâs analogical reasoning illustrates that 3- and 4-year-old children struggle with reasoning by analogy (i.e. glove:hand::sock:___), almost always preferring superficially related âobject matchesâ (:shoe) over ârelational matchesâ (:foot). However, one recent study demonstrated preschoolersâ ability to choose relational matches when a traditional relational-match-to-sample task is embedded in causal scaffolding, framing the target abstract relation as one between beginning and ending states of a causal transformation. Current work aims to discover which factors of causal framing facilitate this boost in early abstract reasoning. In Study 1, we replicate this effect while adapting the transformation to involve two objects, showing that preservation of identity is not necessary for analogical reasoning in a causal context. In Study 2, we explore the replicated effect in a case of non-agentive causation, finding that the causal boost, while still present, is significantly weaker when scaffolding involves a machine vs. an agent. These findings demonstrate that causal framing can be a powerful tool in bolstering childrenâs early abstract reasoning capabilities and show that this enhancing effect is even stronger when an agent holds causal power
Chimpanzees prepare for alternative possible outcomes
When facing uncertainty, humans often build mental models of alternative outcomes. Considering diverging scenarios allows agents to respond adaptively to different actual worlds by developing contingency plans (covering one's bases). In a pre-registered experiment, we tested whether chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) prepare for two mutually exclusive possibilities. Chimpanzees could access two pieces of food, but only if they successfully protected them from a human competitor. In one condition, chimpanzees could be certain about which piece of food the human experimenter would attempt to steal. In a second condition, either one of the food rewards was a potential target of the competitor. We found that chimpanzees were significantly more likely to protect both pieces of food in the second relative to the first condition, raising the possibility that chimpanzees represent and prepare effectively for different possible worlds