136 research outputs found

    What's on your mind? Recent advances in memory detection using the concealed information test

    Get PDF
    Lie detectors can be applied in a wide variety of settings. But this advantage comes with a considerable cost: False positives. The applicability of the Concealed Information Test (CIT) is More limited, yet when it can be applied, the risk of false accusations can be set a priori at a very low level. The CIT assesses the recognition of; critical information that is known only by the examiners and the culprit, for example, the face a an accomplice. Large effects are Obtained with the CIT, whether combined with peripheral, brain, or Motor responses. We see three important challenges for the CIT. First, the false negative rate Of the CIT can be substantial, particularly under :realistic circumstantes. A possible solution Seems to restrict the CIT to highly Salient details. Second, there exist effective faking strategies. Future research will tell whether faking can be detected or even prevented (e.g., Using Overt measures). Third, recognition of critical crime detail's does not necessarily result from criminal activity. It is therefore important to properly embed the CIT in the investigative process, While taking care when drawing conclusions from the test outcome (recognition, not guilt)

    The polygraph and the detection of deception

    Get PDF
    Over the last decade, Europe has seen a marked increase in the use of the polygraph for the detection of deception. Belgium and Finland nowadays regularly use polygraph tests in criminal investigations, and the United Kingdom and the Netherlands have adopted its use in the treatment and monitoring of sex offenders. Still, the use of the polygraph for the detection of deception has been debated in the scientific literature for ages. In this article, we highlight the promises and perils of the use of the polygraph for the detection of deception

    Examining witness interviewing environments

    Get PDF
    The literature on information elicitation in psycholegal settings has predominantly focused on the investigator–interviewee dynamic, with little attention to the environment in which the interview takes place. The present study compared the impact of two interview locations on the disclosure of crime‐related information and perceptions of rapport building. Participants experienced a virtual reality mock crime, and 1 week later were interviewed at either their homes, or a formal room akin to a real‐world police interview room. Participants in the home setting reported feeling more at ease and in control compared to participants interviewed in the formal room. However, we found no differences between conditions on the quantity and quality of information disclosure and participants' perceptions of rapport building. Based on our findings, we found no advantages or disadvantages for conducting witness interviews at their homes. However, these results underscore the practicality of interviewing witnesses outside the police interview room if deemed as more convenien

    Detainee and layperson’s expectations and preferences regarding police interview rooms

    Get PDF
    Emerging research on how suspects perceive the physical environment during investigative interviews yields contrasting findings. While previous studies have suggested that a room made to be physically comfortable may be optimal for interviewing suspects, another study found it can instead lead to higher suspicion of the investigator's intentions. The current study examined current detainees' and general population participants' beliefs about a room that resembled a "typical" interview room, and one decorated to be warm, inviting, and comfortable. Participants also provided descriptive information about their perceptions of police interview environments (e.g., preferences, expectations). We hypothesized that the decorated room would elicit higher ratings of suspicion and wariness compared to the "typical" room. Our findings showed that, overall, participants expected to be interviewed in the "typical" room but preferred the decorated one. Contrary to our expectations, they rated the "typical" room higher on feelings of suspicion than the decorated room. The decorated room also corresponded with what participants reported to be an environment that promotes disclosure. These results bode well for conducting investigative interviews in comfortable environments

    A within-statement baseline comparison for detecting lies

    Get PDF
    To make veracity judgements in individual cases, practitioners may rely on baselining. That is, they may evaluate a statement relative to a baseline statement that is known to be truthful. We investigated whether a within-statement verbal baseline comparison could enhance discriminatory accuracy. Participants (n = 148) read an alibi statement of a mock suspect and provided a veracity judgement regarding a critical two-hour period within the alibi statement. This critical element was either deceptive or truthful and was embedded into an otherwise truthful story. Half of the participants received additional instructions to use the surrounding truthful elements of the statement as a baseline. Instructing participants to make a within-statement baseline comparison did not improve the accuracy of credibility assessments

    The ease of lying

    Get PDF
    Brain imaging studies suggest that truth telling constitutes the default of the human brain and that lying involves intentional suppression of the predominant truth response. By manipulating the truth proportion in the Sheffield lie test, we investigated whether the dominance of the truth response is malleable. Results showed that frequent truth telling made lying more difficult, and that frequent lying made lying easier. These results implicate that (1) the accuracy of lie detection tests may be improved by increasing the dominance of the truth response and that (2) habitual lying makes the lie response more dominant

    Increasing pressure does not benefit lie detection: a reply to Ten Brinke et al. (2015)

    Get PDF
    When passively attending to suspects, observers are poor at distinguishing lies from truths. Deception research has therefore shifted to examining interview styles aimed at eliciting and enhancing deception cues. Based upon a literature review and three empirical studies, ten Brinke, L., Khambatta, P., and Carney, D. R. [2015. Physically scarce (vs. enriched) environments decrease the ability to successfully tell lies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 982–992. doi:10.1037/xge0000103] recommend increasing pressure on interviewees as it would increase lie detection accuracy. In this comment, we argue that these authors (1) misinterpret the literature when concluding that lie detection benefits from increasing pressure on interviewees, and (2) their data do not show that lie detection is more accurate when pressure is increased. In absence of such data, we recommend that increasing pressure on interviewees should be avoided: it hampers the elicitation of valuable information and can lead to false confessions

    Scientific Content Analysis (SCAN) Cannot Distinguish Between Truthful and Fabricated Accounts of a Negative Event

    Get PDF
    The Scientific Content Analysis (SCAN) is a verbal veracity assessment method that is currently used worldwide by investigative authorities. Yet, research investigating the accuracy of SCAN is scarce. The present study tested whether SCAN was able to accurately discriminate between true and fabricated statements. To this end, 117 participants were asked to write down one true and one fabricated statement about a recent negative event that happened in their lives. All statements were analyzed using 11 criteria derived from SCAN. Results indicated that SCAN was not able to correctly classify true and fabricated statements. Lacking empirical support, the application of SCAN in its current form should be discouraged

    Kidney phosphate wasting predicts poor outcome in polycystic kidney disease

    Get PDF
    Background. Patients with autosomal dominant polycystic kidney disease (ADPKD) have disproportionately high levels of fibroblast growth factor 23 (FGF-23) for their chronic kidney disease stage, however only a subgroup develops kidney phosphate wasting. We assessed factors associated with phosphate wasting and hypothesize that it identifies patients with more severe disease and predicts disease progression. Methods. We included 604 patients with ADPKD from a multicenter prospective observational cohort (DIPAK; Developing Intervention Strategies to Halt Progression of Autosomal Dominant Polycystic Kidney Disease) in four university medical centers in the Netherlands. We measured parathyroid hormone (PTH) and total plasma FGF-23 levels, and calculated the ratio of tubular maximum reabsorption rate of phosphate to glomerular filtration rate (TmP/GFR) with &lt;0.8 mmol/L defined as kidney phosphate wasting. We analysed the association of TmP/GFR with estimated GFR (eGFR) decline over time and the risk for a composite kidney outcome (≄30% eGFR decline, kidney failure or kidney replacement therapy). Results. In our cohort (age 48 ± 12 years, 39% male, eGFR 63 ± 28 mL/min/1.73 m 2), 59% of patients had phosphate wasting. Male sex [coefficient -0.2, 95% confidence interval (CI) -0.2; -0.1], eGFR (0.002, 95% CI 0.001; 0.004), FGF-23 (0.1, 95% CI 0.03; 0.2), PTH (-0.2, 95% CI -0.3; -0.06) and copeptin (-0.08, 95% CI -0.1; -0.08) were associated with TmP/GFR. Corrected for PTH, FGF-23 and eGFR, every 0.1 mmol/L decrease in TmP/GFR was associated with a greater eGFR decline of 0.2 mL/min/1.73 m 2/year (95% CI 0.01; 0.3) and an increased hazard ratio of 1.09 (95% CI 1.01; 1.18) of the composite kidney outcome. Conclusion. Our study shows that in patients with ADPKD, phosphate wasting is prevalent and associated with more rapid disease progression. Phosphate wasting may be a consequence of early proximal tubular dysfunction and insufficient suppression of PTH.</p
    • 

    corecore