48 research outputs found
Metacognition in the Prisoner's Dilemma
In this paper, we show ACT-R agents capable of metacognitive reasoning about opponents in the repeated prisoner’s dilemma. Two types of metacognitive agent were developed and compared to a non-metacognitive agent and two fixed-strategy agents. The first type of metacognitive agent (opponent-perspective) takes the perspective of the opponent to anticipate the opponent’s future actions and respond accordingly. The other metacognitive agent (modeler) predicts the opponent’s next move based on the previous moves of the agent and the opponent. The modeler agent achieves better individual outcomes than a non-metacognitive agent and is more successful at encouraging cooperation. The opponent perspective agent, by contrast, fails to achieve these outcomes because it lacks important information about the opponent. These simple agents provide insights regarding modeling of metacognition in more complex tasks
Metacognition in the Prisoner's Dilemma
In this paper, we show ACT-R agents capable of metacognitive reasoning about opponents in the repeated prisoner’s dilemma. Two types of metacognitive agent were developed and compared to a non-metacognitive agent and two fixed-strategy agents. The first type of metacognitive agent (opponent-perspective) takes the perspective of the opponent to anticipate the opponent’s future actions and respond accordingly. The other metacognitive agent (modeler) predicts the opponent’s next move based on the previous moves of the agent and the opponent. The modeler agent achieves better individual outcomes than a non-metacognitive agent and is more successful at encouraging cooperation. The opponent perspective agent, by contrast, fails to achieve these outcomes because it lacks important information about the opponent. These simple agents provide insights regarding modeling of metacognition in more complex tasks
Can I handle the scalpel?:Different views on critical subtask assessment between residents and expert surgeons
Background: Success of surgical procedures largely depends on good judgment and decision making and these skills are mostly taught implicitly in the OR. Cognitive task analysis (CTA) has been used successful to elicit tacit expert knowledge to determine the decision points in a medical procedure and then use this in medical skill instructions. For instructions to be effective, it is also important that they are timed properly, that is, during low mental workload in the resident. Aim(s): As a start for further research, we therefore asked residents to indicate their mental workload during the steps of an orthopaedic procedure. Because in the OR, residents are commonly guided by supervisors, we also investigated which steps supervisors find mentally demanding. Methods: A task analysis of a total hip replacement procedure was constructed. Subsequently we asked both 17 orthopedic surgeons (supervisors) and 21 residents to rate (5-point scale) how much mental effort they invest in each subtask. They were asked to explain ratings above average (>3). Results: Although there were subtasks where supervisors and residents disagreed on the level of attention required, overall, supervisors and residents agreed for most subtasks. High attention was related to subtasks that require decision making skills. However, the reasons were different. In residents, high ratings were associated with subtasks that they found difficult, or where they were anxious about doing it accurately. Supervisors’ ratings were more correlated with the evaluation of crucial, i.e., point-of-no-return steps where quality of execution has important consequences for later steps, end result or complications. Discussion/Conclusion: Supervisors pay more attention to steps that will become important later in the procedure, whereas residents are more anxious about their own performance in crucial steps. It may be useful for supervisors and residents to be aware of this difference in interpretation of “important steps”