6 research outputs found
Visual laterality in the domestic horse (Equus caballus) interacting with humans
Most horses have a side on which they are easier to
handle and a direction they favour when working on a
circle, and recent studies have suggested a correlation
between emotion and visual laterality when horses
observe inanimate objects. As such lateralisation
could provide important clues regarding the horse’s
cognitive processes, we investigated whether horses
also show laterality in association with people. We
gave horses the choice of entering a chute to left or
right, with and without the passive, non-interactive
presence of a person unknown to them. The left eye
was preferred for scanning under both conditions, but
significantly more so when a person was present. Traditionally,
riders handle horses only from the left, so
we repeated the experiment with horses specifically
trained on both sides. Again, there was a consistent
preference for left eye scanning in the presence of
a person, whether known to the horses or not. We
also examined horses interacting with a person,
using both traditionally and bilaterally trained horses.
Both groups showed left eye preference for viewing
the person, regardless of training and test procedure.
For those horses tested under both passive and interactive
conditions, the left eye was preferred significantly
more during interaction. We suggest that most
horses prefer to use their left eye for assessment and
evaluation, and that there is an emotional aspect to
the choice which may be positive or negative, depending
on the circumstances. We believe these results
have important practical implications and that emotional
laterality should be taken into account in training
methods
An analysis of equine round pen training videos posted online: Differences between amateur and professional trainers
Natural Horsemanship is popular among many amateur and professional trainers and as such, has been the subject of recent scientific enquiry. One method commonly adopted by Natural Horsemanship (NH) trainers is that of round pen training (RPT). RPT sessions are usually split into a series of bouts; each including two phases: chasing/flight and chasing offset/flight offset. However, NH training styles are heterogeneous. This study investigated online videos of RPT to explore the characteristics of RPT sessions and test for differences in techniques and outcomes between amateurs and professionals (the latter being defined as those with accompanying online materials that promote clinics, merchandise or a service to the public). From more than 300 candidate videos, we selected sample files for individual amateur (n = 24) and professional (n = 21) trainers. Inclusion criteria were: training at liberty in a Round Pen; more than one bout and good quality video. Sessions or portions of sessions were excluded if the trainer attached equipment, such as a lunge line, directly to the horse or the horse was saddled, mounted or ridden. The number of bouts and duration of each chasing and non-chasing phase were recorded, and the duration of each RPT session was calculated. General weighted regression analysis revealed that, when compared with amateurs, professionals showed fewer arm movements per bout (p<0.05). Poisson regression analysis showed that professionals spent more time looking up at their horses, when transitioning between gaits, than amateurs did (p<0.05). The probability of horses following the trainer was not significantly associated with amount of chasing, regardless of category. Given that, according to some practitioners, the following response is a goal of RPT, this result may prompt caution in those inclined to give chase. The horses handled by professionals showed fewer conflict behaviours (e.g. kicking, biting, stomping, head-tossing, defecating, bucking and attempting to escape), and fewer oral and head movements (e.g. head-lowering, licking and chewing) than those horses handled by amateurs Overall, these findings highlight the need for selectivity when using the internet as an educational source and the importance of trainer skill and excellent timing when using negative reinforcement in horse training