Skip to main content
Article thumbnail
Location of Repository

A century of generalization

By Stefano Ghirlanda and Magnus Enquist

Abstract

We review empirical data from both ethology and psychology about generalization, that is how animals respond to sets of stimuli including familiar as well as novel stimuli. Our main conclusion is that many characteristics of generalization are universal behavioural phenomena, suggesting that they originate from general properties of nervous mechanisms and/or that evolutionary strategies to cope with novelty and variability in stimulation are limited. We also reach the following conclusions. First, patterns of generalization are largely independent of systematic group (evidence is available for insects, fish, amphibians, reptiles, birds and mammals, including humans), behavioural context (feeding, drinking, orientation, etc.), modality (light, sound, etc.) and of whether reaction to stimuli is learned or genetically inherited. Second, two major shapes of the generalization gradient can be identified, corresponding to two types of stimulus dimensions. When changes in stimulation involve a rearrangement of a constant amount of stimulation on the sense organs, the generalization gradient peaks close to familiar stimuli, and peak responding is not much higher than responding to familiar stimuli. When the dimension involves increase or decrease of the intensity of stimulation, the gradient is often monotonic, and responding to some novel stimuli is considerably stronger than responding to familiar stimuli, compared to non-intensity dimensions. Third, rearrangement gradients are better described by Gaussian curves, rather than exponentials as often suggested. Fourth, when several or many familiar stimuli are close to each other interferences occur, giving rise to predictable response biases such as peak shift or supernormality (interference between positive and negative stimuli), and stretched bell shaped curves (several positive stimuli). Lastly, no principle differences between biases referred to as peak shift and biases referred to as supernormal stimulation are found. The paper ends with a discussion of theoretical issues

Topics: Ethology, Animal Cognition, Animal Behavior
Year: 2003
OAI identifier: oai:cogprints.org:5279
Download PDF:
Sorry, we are unable to provide the full text but you may find it at the following location(s):
  • http://cogprints.org/5279/1/gh... (external link)
  • http://cogprints.org/5279/ (external link)
  • Suggested articles

    Citations

    1. (1992). A comparison of generalization functions and frame of reference effects in different training paradigms.
    2. (1958). A new technique for studying spatial generalization with voluntary response.
    3. (1967). A peak shift on a line-tilt continuum.
    4. (1998). Aposematism and gregariousness: the combined effect of group size and coloration on signal repellence.
    5. (1970). Auditory frequency generalization in the goldfish (Carassius auratus).
    6. (1971). Behavioural contrast and peak shift in children.
    7. (1951). Bidirectional gradients in the strength of a generalized voluntary response to stimuli on a visual-spatial dimension.
    8. (1983). Categorical responses to natural and synthesized alarm calls in Goeldi’s monkeys (Callimico goeldii).
    9. (1969). Chapters 9–12. In Learning (Ed.
    10. (1966). Comparison of stimulus generalization following variable-ratio and variableinterval training.
    11. (1980). Conditional gradient displacements: the effects of conditional discrimination training on human auditory frequency generalization.
    12. (1958). Deprivation and generalization. doi
    13. (1971). Differential responding as a function of auditory stimulus intensity without differential reinforcement.
    14. (1972). Discrimination and generalization of complex visual shape variations in pigeons.
    15. (1960). Effect of discrimination training on auditory generalization. doi
    16. (1982). Effects of memory reactivation treatment on postdiscrimination generalization performance in pigeons.
    17. (1962). Egg shell removal by the Black-headed Gull, (Larus r. ridibundus L.) II. The effects of experience on the response to colour.
    18. (1982). Female choice selects for extreme tail length in a widowbird.
    19. (1990). Female preference predates the evolution of the sword in swordtail fish.
    20. (1975). Function and evolution of behaviour.
    21. (1969). Generalisation gradient shape and summation in steady-state tests.
    22. (1997). Generalization and the evolution of symmetry preferences.
    23. (1979). Generalization gradients following differential intradimensional autoshaping.
    24. (1974). Generalization of free-operant avoidance behavior in pigeons. doi
    25. (1969). Generalization of habituation of the GSR to white noise of varying intensities.
    26. (1968). Generalization tests along a light intensity dimension following three S-Rreinforcement contingencies.
    27. (1963). Generalization: I. Generalization gradients from single and multiple stimulus points. II. Generalization of inhibition. doi
    28. (1958). Gradients in response percentage as indices of nonspatial generalization.
    29. (1964). Human neonatal cardiac responses to sound: a power function.
    30. (1985). Human peak shift: Analysis of the effects of three-stimulus discrimination training.
    31. (1979). Influence of acoustic experience on the ontogeny of frequency generalization gradients in the chicken. doi
    32. (1962). Instructions and stimulus categorizing in a measure of stimulus generalization.
    33. (2001). Male biases for male characteristics in females
    34. (1997). Olfactory conditioning in the honey bee, Apis mellifera: effects of odor intensity.
    35. (1971). On the form of stimulus generalization curves for visual intensity.
    36. (1970). Peak shift resulting from a chained reinforcement schedule.
    37. (1965). Physiological and psychological aspects of selective perception.
    38. (1971). Postdiscrimination gradients as a function of variation in reward schedules.
    39. (1973). Postdiscrimination gradients of human subjects on a tone continuum.
    40. (1972). Prediction of the peak shift in pigeons from gradients of excitation and inhibition.
    41. (1993). Preferences of female American goldfinches (emphCarduelis tristis ) for natural and artificial male traits. doi
    42. (1943). Principles of Behaviour.
    43. (1991). Principles of neural science. doi
    44. (1995). Prototype effects and peak shift in categorization.
    45. (1994). Reduced generalization following preexposure: Latent inhibition of common elements or a difference in familiarity?
    46. (1953). Response strength and conditioned stimulus intensity. doi
    47. (1965). Role of adaption level stimulus intensity dynamism.
    48. (1986). Sexual selection for aesthetic traits in species with biparental care.
    49. (1975). Some effects of response-independent reinforcement in auditory generalization gradients. doi
    50. (1965). Some effects of wavelength discrimination on stimulus generalization in the goldfish.
    51. (1975). Steady state data and a quantitative model of operant generalization and discrimination.
    52. (1973). Stimulus control of avoidance behavior in rats following differential or nondifferential pavlovian training along dimensions of the conditioned stimulus.
    53. (1972). Stimulus control of shuttle avoidance in young and adult rats.
    54. (1959). Stimulus generalisation after equal training on three stimuli: a test of the summation hypothesis. doi
    55. (1952). Stimulus generalization after extinction and punishment: an experimental study of displacement.
    56. (1972). Stimulus generalization and discrimination along the click-frequency (flutter) continuum in pigeons.
    57. (1957). Stimulus generalization as a function of age in children.
    58. (1959). Stimulus generalization as a function of level of motivation.
    59. (1964). Stimulus generalization as a function of the number and range of generalization test stimuli.
    60. (1960). Stimulus generalization as a function of the time between training and testing procedures.
    61. (1964). Stimulus generalization in the absence of discrimination factors.
    62. (1950). Stimulus intensity and attention in relation to learning theory.
    63. (1967). Stimulus intensity dynamism and generalization of inhibition in a discriminated lever press avoidance situation.
    64. (1949). Stimulus intensity dynamisn (V) and stimulus generalization. doi
    65. (1973). Stimulus selection.
    66. (1986). Summation and configuration in conditioning of the rabbits nictitating membrane response to compound stimuli. doi
    67. (1999). The development of animal behaviour: a reader.
    68. (1979). The effects of D-9-THC on wavelength generalization in the pigeon.
    69. (1981). the effects of s+ and s− alternation on stimulus control and the peak shift.
    70. (1942). The generalization of approach responses as a function of stimulus intensity and strength of motivation.
    71. (1924). The generalization of conditioned responses: II. The sensory generalization of conditioned responses with varying intensities of tone. doi
    72. (1995). The handbook of brain theory and neural networks.
    73. (1982). The hering gull and its egg. Part II. The responsiveness to egg-features.
    74. (1981). The legacy of Guttman and Kalish doi
    75. (1965). The neural basis of stimulus generalization.
    76. (1958). The relationship between discriminability and generalization: a re-evaluation. doi
    77. (1951). The study of instinct.
    78. (1979). Two determinants of the peak shift in human voluntary stimulus generalization.
    79. (1963). Wave-length generalization as a function of spacing of test stimuli.
    80. (1972). Wavelength generalization and discrimination in the pigeon.

    To submit an update or takedown request for this paper, please submit an Update/Correction/Removal Request.