196 research outputs found
Motor development research : I. The lessons of history revisited (the 18th to the 20th century)
In 1989, Clark and Whitall asked the question “What is motor development”. They were referring to the study of motor development as an academic research enterprise and answered their question primarily by describing four relatively distinct time periods characterized by changes in focus, theories or concepts and methodology. Their last period was named the process-oriented period (1970-1989). In hindsight, it seems clear that their last period could be divided into two separate historical time periods: the information-processing period (1970-1982) and the dynamical systems period (1982-2000). In the present paper, we briefly revisit the first three periods defined by Clark and Whitall, and expand and elaborate on the two periods from 1970 to the turn of the century. Each period is delineated by key papers and the major changes in focus, theories or concepts and methodology. Major findings about motor development are also described from some papers as a means of showing the progression of knowledge
Construct validity of the pictorial scale of perceived movement skill competence
Objectives: The Pictorial Scale of Perceived Movement Skill Competence (PMSC) assesses young children\u27s perceptions of movement skill competence: 12 perceived Fundamental Movement skills (FMS; based on the Test of Gross Motor Development 2nd edition TGMD-2) and six Active Play activities (e.g. cycling). The main study purpose was to assess whether children\u27s movement perception scores fit within the imposed constructs of Active Play and FMS by testing the latent structure and construct validity of the PMSC. Design: Construct validation study. Methods: Participants were part of the Melbourne Infant Feeding, Activity and Nutrition Trial (InFANT). The latent structure of the PMSC responses was tested through confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and Bayesian Structural Equation Modeling (BSEM). Internal consistency was conducted using polychoric correlation-based alphas. Results: The 303 children (boys 53.1%, n = 161) were aged 4-5 years (M = 4.7, SD = 0.46). The final model had an 18 item 3-factor solution with good fit indices (using CFA and BSEM). Factors were: Active Play (Bike, Board Paddle, Climb, Skate/Blade, Scooter, and Swim), Object Control - Hand Skills (Bounce, Catch, Hit, Throw), and FMS skills with a leg action (Gallop, Hop, Jump, Leap, Run, Step Slide, Kick, Roll). Alpha reliability values were: Active Play (0.78), Object Control-Hand Skills (0.76) and FMS-Dynamic Leg (0.84). Conclusion: Young children can distinguish between movement perceptions. The factors reflect the hypothesized structure in terms of FMS being distinguished from Active Play. Further research should investigate how and if these constructs change in children over time
Motor competence and its effect on positive developmental trajectories of health
In 2008, Stodden and colleagues took a unique developmental approach toward addressing the potential
role of motor competence in promoting positive or negative trajectories of physical activity, health-related fitness, and weight status. The conceptual model proposed synergistic relationships among physical activity, motor competence, perceived motor competence, health-related physical fit ness, and obesity with associations hypothesized to strengthen over time. At the time the model was proposed, limited evidence was available to support or refute the model hypotheses. Over the past 6 years, the number of investigations exploring these relationships has increased significantly. Thus, it is an appropriate time to examine
published data that directly or indirectly relate to specific pathways noted in the conceptual model. Evidence indi cates that motor competence is positively associated with perceived competence and multiple aspects of health (i.e., physical activity, cardiorespiratory fitness, muscular strength, muscular endurance, and a healthy weight status). However, questions related to the increased strength of associations across time and antecedent/consequent mech anisms remain. An individual’s physical and psychological development is a complex and multifaceted process that synergistically evolves across time. Understanding the
most salient factors that influence health and well-being and how relationships among these factors change across time is a critical need for future research in this area. This knowledge could aid in addressing the declining levels of physical activity and fitness along with the increasing rates of obesity across childhood and adolescence.4811-99FE-2ECD | Luis Paulo Rodriguesinfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersio
Reflections on motor development research across the 20th century : six empirical studies that changed the field
Motor development research has had a rich history over the 20th century with a wide array of scientists contributing to a broad and deep body of literature. Just like the process of development, progress within the field has been non-linear, with rapid periods of growth occurring after the publication of key research articles that changed how we conceptualized and explored motor development. These publications provided new ways to consider developmental issues and, as a result, ignited change in our theoretical and empirical approaches within the field of motor development and the broader field of developmental psychology. In this paper, we outline and discuss six pioneering studies that we consider significant in their impact and in the field's evolution, in order of publication: Halverson, 1931; Wild, 1938; Gibson & Walk, 1960; Connolly, Brown, & Bassett, 1968; Thelen & Fisher, 1982; Thelen & Ulrich, 1991. We have limited this review to empirical papers only. Together, they offer insight into what motor development research is, where it came from, why it matters, and what it has achieved
Physical fitness spurts in pre-adolescent boys and girls: Timing, intensity and sequencing
We aim to (1) estimate age of attainment of the peak mid-growth spurt in stature (age-at-peak MGS) in
pre-adolescent boys and girls; (2) identify the timing, intensity, and sequences of physical fitness (PF)
spurts aligned by the age-at-peak MGS; and (3) identify any sex differences in PF spurts aligned by age-at peak MGS. The sample included 180 Portuguese children (90 girls) aged 6 to 10 years at study entry who
were followed annually for 4 years. Height, health-, and performance-related PF were assessed. Age-at peak MGS and PF spurts were estimated using a non-smooth mathematical procedure. Boys’ and girls’
age-at-peak MGS occurred at 7.8 ± 0.47 years and 8.0 ± 0.72 years, respectively. PF spurts’ timing aligned
by age-at-peak MGS were as follows: (1) before age-at-peak MGS: boys – static strength, aerobic capacity,
explosive leg strength, and flexibility; girls – speed, agility, aerobic capacity, and upper body strength; (2)
coincident with age-at-peak MGS: girls – explosive leg strength and flexibility; (3) after age-at-peak MGS:
boys – abdominal strength, upper body strength, agility, and speed; girls – abdominal strength and static
strength. Boys and girls attained their MGS at relatively similar ages. However, the timing and sequences
of PF spurts, aligned on age-at-MGS, were different between boys and girls.info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersio
Motor Competence as Key to Support Healthy Development of 3- to 5-Year-Old Children:An Expert Statement on Behalf of the International Motor Development Research Consortium
The first years of life are an optimal time for developing motor competence. However, the evidence regarding motor competence in early childhood is fragmented and needs to be clearly synthesized and presented. To establish effective evidence-based decision making in research, practice, and policy for the early years, this expert statement, on behalf of the International Motor Development Research Consortium, draws together what is currently known about 3- to 5-year-old children on (a) how skilled are children around the world, (b) the link between motor competence and healthy developmental outcomes, and (c) the capacity to improve children’s motor competence through intervention. This expert statement presents a summary of recent evidence for each of these specific points, followed by recommendations for researchers, practitioners, and policymakers
Correction to: Through the looking glass: a systematic review of longitudinal evidence, providing new insight for motor competence and health
Tables 6, 7, 11 and 12 each include a header cell, near the
top, stating the wording “Proportion of sig. analyses (without
studies with > 4 comparisons)”. Table 6 also features a
header cell at the bottom, stating “Proportion of sig total
Locomotor/ Coordination /Stability analyses without studies
with > 4 comparisons”.info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersio
A systematic review of longitudinal and experimental evidence providing new insight for motor competence and health
In 2008, a conceptual model explaining the role of motor competence (MC) on children’s physical activity (PA), health-related fitness, weight
status and perceived MC was published by Stodden et al. The purpose of this review is to systematically compile mediation, longitudinal and
experimental evidence in support of this model. Searches were undertaken for each pathway of interest using six relevant databases.
Potential articles were identified though abstract and title checking (N = 585), then screened (n = 152), with 43 articles identified for
extraction. Studies needed to: be original, peer-reviewed, include typically developing children and adolescents first assessed between 2
and 18 years and objective assessment of gross MC and at least one other model variable. Strength of evidence was calculated for each
pathway in both directions by dividing the proportion of studies indicating a significantly positive pathway in the hypothesized direction by the
total amount of studies investigating that pathway. Classifications were no association (0–33 %), indeterminate/inconsistent (34–59 %), or a
positive “+” or negative “-” association (≥60 %). The latter category was classified as strong evidence (i.e., ++ or –) when four or more
studies found an association. If the total number of studies in a domain of interest were three or less, this was considered insufficient
evidence. There was strong evidence in both directions for a negative association between MC and weight status. There was indeterminate
evidence between MC and fitness and indeterminate evidence from MC to PA and no evidence for the reverse. There was insufficient
evidence for the MC to perceived MC pathway. There was strong positive evidence for the fitness-mediated pathway in both directions.
There was indeterminate evidence for the perceived MC-mediated pathway from PA to MC and no evidence for the reverse. To test the
whole model, the field needs robust longitudinal studies with multiple time points, including all variables in the model and accounting for
confounding factors.N.C.V is supported by the Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education Personel – CAPESPrint
Brazil. V.P.L is supported by national funding through the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology, I.P., under project
UID04045/2020 L.P.R is partially supported by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology, I.P. under Project
UID/DTP/04045/2019.info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersio
Host phenotype characteristics and MC1R in relation to early-onset basal cell carcinoma.
Basal cell carcinoma (BCC) incidence is increasing, particularly among adults under the age of 40 years. Pigment-related characteristics are associated with BCC in older populations, but epidemiologic studies among younger individuals and analyses of phenotype-genotype interactions are limited. We examined self-reported phenotypes and melanocortin 1 receptor gene (MC1R) variants in relation to early-onset BCC. BCC cases (n=377) and controls with benign skin conditions (n=390) under the age of 40 years were identified through Yale's Dermatopathology database. Factors most strongly associated with early-onset BCC were skin reaction to first summer sun for 1 hour (severe sunburn vs. tan odds ratio (OR)=12.27, 95% confidence interval (CI)=4.08-36.94) and skin color (very fair vs. olive OR=11.06, 95% CI=5.90-20.74). Individuals with two or more MC1R non-synonymous variants were 3.59 times (95% CI=2.37-5.43) more likely to have BCC than those without non-synonymous variants. All host characteristics and MC1R were more strongly associated with multiple BCC case status (37% of cases) than a single BCC case status. MC1R, number of moles, skin reaction to first summer sun for 1 hour, and hair and skin color were independently associated with BCC. BCC risk conferred by MC1R tended to be stronger among those with darker pigment phenotypes, traditionally considered to be at low risk of skin cancer
Recommended from our members
Motor Competence and its Effect on Positive Developmental Trajectories of Health
In 2008, Stodden and colleagues took a unique developmental approach toward addressing the potential role of motor competence in promoting positive or negative trajectories of physical activity, health-related fitness, and weight status. The conceptual model proposed synergistic relationships among physical activity, motor competence, perceived motor competence, health-related physical fitness, and obesity with associations hypothesized to strengthen over time. At the time the model was proposed, limited evidence was available to support or refute the model hypotheses. Over the past 6 years, the number of investigations exploring these relationships has increased significantly. Thus, it is an appropriate time to examine published data that directly or indirectly relate to specific pathways noted in the conceptual model. Evidence indicates that motor competence is positively associated with perceived competence and multiple aspects of health (i.e., physical activity, cardiorespiratory fitness, muscular strength, muscular endurance, and a healthy weight status). However, questions related to the increased strength of associations across time and antecedent/consequent mechanisms remain. An individual’s physical and psychological development is a complex and multifaceted process that synergistically evolves across time. Understanding the most salient factors that influence health and well-being and how relationships among these factors change across time is a critical need for future research in this area. This knowledge could aid in addressing the declining levels of physical activity and fitness along with the increasing rates of obesity across childhood and adolescence
- …