2,107 research outputs found

    Correlation of Automorphism Group Size and Topological Properties with Program-size Complexity Evaluations of Graphs and Complex Networks

    Get PDF
    We show that numerical approximations of Kolmogorov complexity (K) applied to graph adjacency matrices capture some group-theoretic and topological properties of graphs and empirical networks ranging from metabolic to social networks. That K and the size of the group of automorphisms of a graph are correlated opens up interesting connections to problems in computational geometry, and thus connects several measures and concepts from complexity science. We show that approximations of K characterise synthetic and natural networks by their generating mechanisms, assigning lower algorithmic randomness to complex network models (Watts-Strogatz and Barabasi-Albert networks) and high Kolmogorov complexity to (random) Erdos-Renyi graphs. We derive these results via two different Kolmogorov complexity approximation methods applied to the adjacency matrices of the graphs and networks. The methods used are the traditional lossless compression approach to Kolmogorov complexity, and a normalised version of a Block Decomposition Method (BDM) measure, based on algorithmic probability theory.Comment: 15 2-column pages, 20 figures. Forthcoming in Physica A: Statistical Mechanics and its Application

    The Busy Beaver Competition: a historical survey

    Full text link
    Tibor Rado defined the Busy Beaver Competition in 1962. He used Turing machines to give explicit definitions for some functions that are not computable and grow faster than any computable function. He put forward the problem of computing the values of these functions on numbers 1, 2, 3, ... More and more powerful computers have made possible the computation of lower bounds for these values. In 1988, Brady extended the definitions to functions on two variables. We give a historical survey of these works. The successive record holders in the Busy Beaver Competition are displayed, with their discoverers, the date they were found, and, for some of them, an analysis of their behavior.Comment: 70 page

    The Fisheries Analysis System (FAS): Creel Survey and Lake Analyses

    Get PDF
    F-69-R(1-3)Report issued on: October 1990Final report of Project F-69-R(1-3), Data Base Management and Analysis of Fisheries in Impoundments, conducted under of memorandum of understanding between Illinois Department of Conservation and the University of Illinois, supported through Fed(TRUNCATED

    Algorithmic Complexity for Short Binary Strings Applied to Psychology: A Primer

    Full text link
    Since human randomness production has been studied and widely used to assess executive functions (especially inhibition), many measures have been suggested to assess the degree to which a sequence is random-like. However, each of them focuses on one feature of randomness, leading authors to have to use multiple measures. Here we describe and advocate for the use of the accepted universal measure for randomness based on algorithmic complexity, by means of a novel previously presented technique using the the definition of algorithmic probability. A re-analysis of the classical Radio Zenith data in the light of the proposed measure and methodology is provided as a study case of an application.Comment: To appear in Behavior Research Method

    The Busy Beaver Competition: a historical survey

    Get PDF
    70 pagesTibor Rado defined the Busy Beaver Competition in 1962. He used Turing machines to give explicit definitions for some functions that are not computable and grow faster than any computable function. He put forward the problem of computing the values of these functions on numbers 1, 2, 3, ... More and more powerful computers have made possible the computation of lower bounds for these values. In 1988, Brady extended the definitions to functions on two variables. We give a historical survey of these works. The successive record holders in the Busy Beaver Competition are displayed, with their discoverers, the date they were found, and, for some of them, an analysis of their behavior

    Busy beaver machines and the observant otter heuristic

    Get PDF
    The busy beaver problem is to find the maximum number of non-zero characters that can be printed by an n-state Turing machine of a particular type. A critical step in the solution of this problem is to determine whether or not a given n-state Turing machine halts on a blank input. Given the enormous output sizes that can be produced by some small machines, it becomes critical to have appropriate methods for dealing with the exponential behaviour of both terminating and nonterminating machines. In this paper, we investigate a heuristic which can be used to greatly accelerateexecution of this class of machines. This heuristic, which we call the observant otter, is based on the detection of patterns earlier in the execution trace. We describe our implementation of this method and report various experimental results based on it, including showing how it can be used to evaluate all known 'monster' machines, including some whose naive execution would take around 10^36,534 steps

    Approximations of Algorithmic and Structural Complexity Validate Cognitive-behavioural Experimental Results

    Full text link
    We apply methods for estimating the algorithmic complexity of sequences to behavioural sequences of three landmark studies of animal behavior each of increasing sophistication, including foraging communication by ants, flight patterns of fruit flies, and tactical deception and competition strategies in rodents. In each case, we demonstrate that approximations of Logical Depth and Kolmogorv-Chaitin complexity capture and validate previously reported results, in contrast to other measures such as Shannon Entropy, compression or ad hoc. Our method is practically useful when dealing with short sequences, such as those often encountered in cognitive-behavioural research. Our analysis supports and reveals non-random behavior (LD and K complexity) in flies even in the absence of external stimuli, and confirms the "stochastic" behaviour of transgenic rats when faced that they cannot defeat by counter prediction. The method constitutes a formal approach for testing hypotheses about the mechanisms underlying animal behaviour.Comment: 28 pages, 7 figures and 2 table
    corecore