Article thumbnail
Location of Repository

Specialization Does Not Predict Individual Efficiency in an Ant

By Anna Dornhaus

Abstract

The ecological success of social insects is often attributed to an increase in efficiency achieved through division of labor between workers in a colony. Much research has therefore focused on the mechanism by which a division of labor is implemented, i.e., on how tasks are allocated to workers. However, the important assumption that specialists are indeed more efficient at their work than generalist individuals—the “Jack-of-all-trades is master of none” hypothesis—has rarely been tested. Here, I quantify worker efficiency, measured as work completed per time, in four different tasks in the ant Temnothorax albipennis: honey and protein foraging, collection of nest-building material, and brood transports in a colony emigration. I show that individual efficiency is not predicted by how specialized workers were on the respective task. Worker efficiency is also not consistently predicted by that worker's overall activity or delay to begin the task. Even when only the worker's rank relative to nestmates in the same colony was used, specialization did not predict efficiency in three out of the four tasks, and more specialized workers actually performed worse than others in the fourth task (collection of sand grains). I also show that the above relationships, as well as median individual efficiency, do not change with colony size. My results demonstrate that in an ant species without morphologically differentiated worker castes, workers may nevertheless differ in their ability to perform different tasks. Surprisingly, this variation is not utilized by the colony—worker allocation to tasks is unrelated to their ability to perform them. What, then, are the adaptive benefits of behavioral specialization, and why do workers choose tasks without regard for whether they can perform them well? We are still far from an understanding of the adaptive benefits of division of labor in social insects

Topics: Research Article
Publisher: Public Library of Science
OAI identifier: oai:pubmedcentral.nih.gov:2586388
Provided by: PubMed Central

Suggested articles

Citations

  1. A (1776) The wealth of nations.
  2. (2003). A fitness cost of learning ability
  3. (2006). Adaptation, genetic drift, pleiotropy, and history in the evolution of bee foraging behavior.
  4. (2004). An operating cost of learning in Drosophila melanogaster.
  5. (1989). Behavioral flexibility in age castes of the ant Pheidole dentata.
  6. (2007). Behavioral genomics of honeybee foraging and nest defense.
  7. BeshersSN,HuangZY,OonoY,RobinsonGY(2001) Socialinhibitionand the regulation of temporal polyethism in honey bees.
  8. (1979). Bumblebee economics. Cambridge (Massachusetts):
  9. (1995). Caste allocation in litter Pheidole: lessons from plant defense theory.
  10. (1980). Caste and division of labor in leaf-cutter ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae: Atta) II. The ergonomic optimization of leaf cutting.
  11. (1978). Caste and ecology in the social insects.
  12. (2005). Caste evolution and ecology: a special worker for novel prey.
  13. (1993). Caste specialization in food storage in the dimorphic ant Colobopsis nipponicus (Wheeler).
  14. (1987). Causes of ecological success: the case of the ants.
  15. (2006). Colony size affects collective decisionmaking in the ant Temnothorax albipennis.
  16. (1995). Colony size as a buffer against seasonality: Bergmann’s rule in social insects.
  17. (2002). Deciding to learn: modulation of learning flights in honeybees, Apis mellifera.
  18. (2006). Decision-making by small and large house-hunting ant colonies: one size fits all.
  19. (2004). Defensive behavior of honey bees: organization, genetics, and comparisons with other bees.
  20. (1997). Division of labor between undertaker specialists and other middle-aged workers in honey bee colonies.
  21. (2006). Division of labour and colony efficiency in social insects: effects of interactions between genetic architecture, colony kin structure and rate of perturbations.
  22. (2005). Division of labour and worker size polymorphism in ant colonies: the impact of social and genetic factors.
  23. (1997). Ecology, evolution, and division of labour in social insects.
  24. (2002). Evolving the division of labour: generalists, specialists, and task allocation.
  25. (2004). Experimental evidence for effective and altruistic colony defence against natural predators by soldiers of the gall-forming aphid Pemphigus spyrothecae (Hemiptera: Pemphigidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 27: 421–430. PLoS Biology | www.plosbiology.org
  26. (1985). Fire ant polymorphism: the ergonomics of brood production.
  27. (1999). Flower constancy, insect psychology, and plant evolution.
  28. (1988). Flower handling by bumblebees: a comparison of specialists and generalists.
  29. (1990). Forager specialization and the control of nest repair in Polybia occidentalis Olivier (Hymenoptera,
  30. (1997). Foraging dynamics of bumle bees: correlates of movements within and between plant species.
  31. (2004). Foraging for work and age-based polyethism: the roles of age and previous experience on task choice in ants.
  32. (1991). Genetic differences in learning behavior in honeybees (Apis mellifera capensis).
  33. (2008). How experienced individuals contribute to an improvement in collective performance in ants.
  34. (2004). Improvement in collective performance with experience in ants.
  35. (2008). Individual and collective cognition in ants and other insects (Hymenoptera: Formicidae).
  36. (2007). Individual experience alone can generate lasting division of labor in ants.
  37. (2001). Individual vs. social complexity, with particular reference to ant colonies.
  38. (2007). Interaction rate informs harvester ant task decisions.
  39. (2006). Intercolony variation in learning performance of a wild British bumblebee population (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Bombus terrestris audax).
  40. (1992). Landmark learning and guidance in insects.
  41. (1997). Learning and task interference by corpseremoval specialists in honey bee colonies.
  42. (2007). Learning reward expectations in honeybees.
  43. (2008). Life history of learning: performance curves of honeybees in settings that minimize the role of learning.
  44. (2001). Models of division of labor in social insects.
  45. (2008). Multimodal signals enhance decision making in foraging bumble-bees.
  46. (1993). Nest defense and early production of the major workers in the dimorphic ant Colobopsis nipponicus (Wheeler)
  47. (2004). Normalized mutual entropy in biology: quantifying division of labor.
  48. (2007). Pollen foraging: learning a complex motor skill by bumblebees (Bombus terrestris).
  49. (2007). Reconnaissance and latent learning in ants.
  50. (1992). Regulation of division of labor in insect societies.
  51. Rissing SW (2005) Division of foraging labor in ants can mediate demands for food and safety.
  52. (1999). Self-assembly, self-organization and division of labour.
  53. (1998). Sensorimotor learning in bumblebees: long term retention and reversal training.
  54. (1989). Social organization in the ant Pheidole dentata: Physical and temporal caste ratios lack ecological correlates.
  55. (2005). Sociogenomics: social life in molecular terms.
  56. (1995). Spatial relationships within nests of the ant Leptothorax unifasciatus (Latr.) and their implications for the division of labour.
  57. (2004). Sucrose responsiveness and behavioral plasticity in honey bees (Apis mellifera).
  58. (1993). Task allocation in ant colonies within variable environments (a study of temporal polyethism,
  59. (1990). The ants:
  60. (2007). The correlation of learning speed and natural foraging success in bumble-bees.
  61. (2006). The fire ants.
  62. (1976). The foraging specializations of individual bumblebees.
  63. (2006). The genetic architecture of sucrose responsiveness in the honeybee (Apis mellifera L.).
  64. (1987). The guard honey bee: ontogeny and behavioural variability of workers performing a specialized task.
  65. (1986). The organization of work in Polybia occidentalis: costs and benefits of specialization in a social wasp.
  66. (1996). The organization of work in social insect colonies.
  67. (2002). The relationship between the distribution of worker sizes and new worker production in the ant Formica neorufibarbis.
  68. (2006). The role of mechanosensory input in flower handling efficiency and learning by Manduca sexta.
  69. (1999). The role of orientation flights on homing performance in honeybees.
  70. (1985). The sociogenesis of insect colonies.
  71. (2001). The use of edges in visual navigation by the ant Leptothorax albipennis.
  72. (2007). Traplining in bumblebees (Bombus impatiens): a foraging strategy’s ontogeny and the importance of spatial reference memory in short-range foraging.
  73. (1999). Undertaking specialization in the desert leafcutter ant Acromyrmex versicolor.
  74. (2002). Weidenmu ¨ller A
  75. (2008). Why don’t all workers work? Colony size and workload during emigrations in the ant Temnothorax albipennis.
  76. (2007). Worker caste determination in the army ant Eciton burchellii.
  77. (1992). Worker castes and adaptive demography.

To submit an update or takedown request for this paper, please submit an Update/Correction/Removal Request.