Location of Repository

The effects of trait emotional intelligence and sociobiographical variables on communicative anxiety and foreign language anxiety among adult multilinguals: A review and empirical investigation

By Jean-Marc Dewaele, K.V. Petrides and A. Furnham

Abstract

This study considered the effects of trait emotional intelligence (trait EI; Petrides & Furnham, 2001) and sociobiographical variables (age, gender, education level, number of languages known, age of onset of acquisition, context of acquisition, frequency of use, socialization, network of interlocutors, self-perceived proficiency) on communicative anxiety (CA) in the first, and foreign language anxiety (FLA) in the second, third, and fourth languages of 464 multilingual individuals, in five different situations (speaking with friends, colleagues, strangers, on the phone, and in public). Data were collected via web-based questionnaires. Participants were divided into three groups based on their trait EI scores (low, average, high). Non-parametric statistical analyses revealed a consistent pattern of results across languages and situations. Higher levels of trait EI corresponded to significantly lower CA/FLA scores. Participants who started learning the L2 and L3 at a younger age also suffered less from FLA. Purely classroom-based language instruction was found to be linked to higher levels of FLA compared to instruction that also involved extracurricular use of the language. The knowledge of more languages, a higher frequency of use, a stronger socialization in a language, a larger network of interlocutors and a higher level of self-perceived proficiency in a language were also linked to lower levels of CA/FLA

Topics: alc
Publisher: Wiley
Year: 2008
OAI identifier: oai:eprints.bbk.ac.uk.oai2:765

Suggested articles

Preview

Citations

  1. (2006). A critique of recent arguments against the critical period hypothesis. In
  2. (1993). A student’s contributions to second language learning: Part II. Affective variables. doi
  3. (2005). Age and sex differences in willingness to communicate, communication apprehension and self-perceived competence. doi
  4. (2006). Age and the rate of foreign language learning. doi
  5. (1996). Anxiety about foreign language learning among high school women. doi
  6. (2004). Anxiety and predictors of performance in the foreign language classroom. doi
  7. (1989). Anxiety and second language learning: Towards a theoretical clarification. doi
  8. (2006). Anxiety and speaking English as a second language. doi
  9. (2005). Anxiety and the true beginner—false beginner dynamic in beginning French and Spanish classes. Foreign Language Annals, doi
  10. (2002). Background to the L2 user.
  11. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for an accessibility model of emotional selfreport. doi
  12. (1998). Benefits of multisensory structured language instruction for at-risk foreign language learners: A comparison study of high school Spanish students. doi
  13. (1997). Biases in self-ratings of second language proficiency: The role of language anxiety. doi
  14. (2004). Blistering barnacles! What language do multilinguals swear in?! doi
  15. (1998). Conceptualizing willingness to communicate in a L2: A situational model of L2 confidence and affiliation. doi
  16. (2007). Context and L2 users’ pragmatic development. In
  17. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multi matrix. doi
  18. (2000). Correlates of anxiety at three stages of the foreign language learning process. doi
  19. (2006). Critical period or language learning difficulties: Are there two types of adult foreign language learners? In
  20. (2005). Development and validation of a self-report measure of emotional intelligence as a multidimensional trait domain. doi
  21. Diachronic and/or synchronic variation? The acquisition of sociolinguistic competence in L2 French. doi
  22. (2002). Does a self-report measure for emotional intelligence assess something different than general intelligence? Personality and Individual Differences, doi
  23. (2002). Emotion vocabulary in interlanguage. doi
  24. (2004). Emotional intelligence is. doi
  25. (2004). Emotional intelligence. doi
  26. (2005). Emotions and multilingualism. Cambridge, MA: doi
  27. (2006). Expressing anger in multiple languages. In A. Pavlenko (Ed.), Bilingual minds: Emotional experience, expression, and representation
  28. (1999). Extraversion: The unloved variable in applied linguistic research. doi
  29. (1999). Factors associated with foreign language anxiety. doi
  30. (1996). Foreign language anxiety and language performance: A study of learner anxiety in beginning, intermediate, and advanced-level college students of Japanese. doi
  31. (1986). Foreign language classroom anxiety. doi
  32. (1991). Foreign language learning difficulties: Affective or native language aptitude differences? doi
  33. (1995). How does anxiety affect second language learning? A reply to Sparks and Ganschow. doi
  34. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York: doi
  35. Individual differences in L2 fluency: The effect of neurobiological correlates.
  36. Interindividual variation in self-perceived oral communicative competence of English L2 users. In doi
  37. (2003). Introduction: Towards a dynamic model of language socialization. In
  38. (1991). Investigating language class anxiety using the focused essay technique. doi
  39. (2005). Investigating the psychological and the emotional dimensions in instructed language learning: Obstacles and possibilities. doi
  40. (2000). It ain’t over ’til it’s over: On foreign language anxiety, first language deficits, and the confounding of variables. doi
  41. (2001). Language anxiety and achievement. doi
  42. (1999). Language anxiety: A review of the research for language teachers. In doi
  43. (1991). Language anxiety: Its relationship to other anxieties and to processing in native and second language. doi
  44. (2002). Language learning and perfectionism: Anxious and non-anxious language learners’ reactions to their own oral performance. doi
  45. (2007). Language learning and teaching as social inter-action (pp. doi
  46. (2007). Linguıstische vlotheid, accuraatheid en complexiteit in de verwerving en de verwerking van een tweede taal [Linguistic fluency, accuracy and complexity in the acquisition and processing of a second language]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
  47. (1989). Lost in translation: A life in a new language.
  48. (2001). Metalinguistic awareness in multilinguals: Implicit and explicit grammatical awareness and its relationship with language experience and language attainment. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
  49. (1991). Methods and results in the study of foreign language anxiety: A review of the literature. doi
  50. (2000). On the dimensional structure of emotional intelligence. doi
  51. (1985). Personality and individual differences: A natural science approach. doi
  52. (1999). Personality and social behavior.
  53. (2000). Personality and speech production: A pilot study of second language learners. doi
  54. (1999). Personality continuity and change across the life course. In doi
  55. (2003). Personality traits (2nd ed). Cambridge: doi
  56. (1996). Personality, attitudes, and affect as predictors of second language communication. doi
  57. Predicting language learners’ grades in the L1, L2, L3 and L4: The effect of some psychological and sociocognitive variables. doi
  58. (2006). Predicting resistance to stress: Incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence over alexithymia and optimism.
  59. (1986). Preliminary evidence for the reliability and validity of a foreign language anxiety scale. doi
  60. (2002). Psychological and sociodemographic correlates of communicative anxiety in L2 and L3 production. doi
  61. (1976). Psychoticism as a dimension of personality. London: Hodder and Stoughton. doi
  62. (2003). Revisiting the predictive validity of emotional intelligence: Self-report versus ability-based measures. doi
  63. (1988). Self-report as an approach to measuring communication competence. doi
  64. (2002). Sex and age effects on willingness to communicate, anxiety, perceived competence, and L2 motivation among junior high school French immersion students. doi
  65. (2004). Should we trust Web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about Internet questionnaires. doi
  66. (2002). Social networks from a sociolinguistic perspective: The relationship between characteristics of the social networks of bilingual adolescents and their language proficiency. doi
  67. (2005). Sociodemographic, psychological and politico-cultural correlates in Flemish students’ attitudes toward French and English. doi
  68. (2007). Strategic processing in grammar learning: Do multilinguals use more strategies? doi
  69. (2004). Teachers’ role, learners’ gender differences, and FL anxiety among seventh-grade students studying English as a FL. doi
  70. (2007). The effect of multilingualism and socio-situational factors on communicative anxiety and foreign language anxiety of mature language learners. doi
  71. (2005). The effect of type of acquisition context on perception and self-reported use of swearwords in the L2, L3, L4 and L5. doi
  72. (2004). The emotional force of swearwords and taboo words in the speech of multilinguals. doi
  73. (2008). The emotional weight of “I love you” in multilinguals’ languages. doi
  74. (1975). The Eysenck personality questionnaire. London: Hodder and Stoughton. doi
  75. (1998). The g factor. doi
  76. (2007). The happy personality: Mediational role of trait emotional intelligence. doi
  77. (2004). The influence of attitudes and affect on willingness to communicate and second language communication. doi
  78. (2007). The location of trait emotional intelligence in personality factor space. doi
  79. (2005). The psychology of the language learner. Individual differences in second language acquisition. doi
  80. (1987). The role of anxiety in second language performance of language drop-outs
  81. (2002). The role of foreign language anxiety and students’ expectations in foreign language learning. doi
  82. (2000). The role of gender and immersion in communication and second language orientations. doi
  83. (2000). The role of individual and social variables in oral task performance. doi
  84. (2006). The role of trait emotional intelligence in a gender-specific model of organizational variables. doi
  85. (2004). The role of trait emotional intelligence in academic performance and deviant behavior at school. Personality and Individual Differences, doi
  86. (2003). The stability of general foreign language classroom anxiety across English and French. doi
  87. (1994). The subtle effects of language anxiety on cognitive processing in the second language. doi
  88. (2001). Tongue-tied in a foreign language. doi
  89. (2003). Trait emotional intelligence: Behavioural validation in two studies of emotion recognition and reactivity to mood induction. doi
  90. (2007). Trait emotional intelligence: Moving forward in the field of EI. In doi
  91. (2001). Trait emotional intelligence: Psychometric investigation with reference to established trait taxonomies. doi
  92. Web questionnaire on bilingualism and emotion.
  93. (2005). What does the Critical Period really mean? In
  94. (2007). Willingness to communicate in the second language: Understanding the decision to speak as a volitional process. doi

To submit an update or takedown request for this paper, please submit an Update/Correction/Removal Request.