Location of Repository

Age and schooling effects on the development of early literacy and related skills

By Anna Julie Cunningham

Abstract

There is evidence to suggest that age (natural maturation and informal\ud experience) and schooling (formal instruction at school) have differing effects on the\ud development of cognitive skills between the ages of 5 and 7. There is also evidence that\ud children who start learning how to read later in childhood make faster progress than\ud those who start earlier in childhood. However, previous studies on reading development\ud have typically confounded age with length of schooling. The current thesis investigates\ud the separate role of each on the development of early literacy and related skills by\ud comparing matched groups of children differing in either exposure to formal reading\ud instruction or chronological age. Two longitudinal studies are presented, with seven key\ud findings.\ud Chapter 2 presents a cross-sectional study (n = 93) comparing a group of later-schooled\ud 7 year olds with two control groups at the beginning of their first year of\ud reading instruction (earlier-schooled ‘reading controls’ and earlier-schooled ‘age\ud controls’). First, it was shown that vocabulary and short-term verbal memory skills\ud developed with age, not schooling. Second, it was found that phoneme awareness can\ud develop in the absence of formal reading instruction, and that this was more likely to\ud happen in older than younger children.\ud Chapter 3 presents a longitudinal study (n = 61) following the progress of the\ud first two groups from chapter 2; a group of Steiner-educated 7 years olds (later-schooled\ud group) and a group of standard-educated 4 year olds (earlier-schooled group) during\ud their first two years of formal literacy instruction. Results showed that the older age and\ud superior reading-related skills of the first group did not lead to faster progress in\ud reading, and in fact this group showed worse progress in spelling. The good progress of\ud the earlier-schooled group was attributed to more consistent and high quality phonics\ud instruction.\ud By comparing the predictors of reading and spelling in the two groups presented\ud in chapter 3, chapter 4 showed that the skills underlying literacy development were\ud similar in older and younger beginning readers, but that instructional emphasis on letter-sound\ud knowledge in the earlier-schooled group reduced its power as a predictor.\ud Chapter 5 used mediation analyses to show that letter-sound knowledge led to\ud phonological awareness, which in turn led to reading in the earlier-schooled children, an\ud effect which was attributed to the method of synthetic phonics instruction administered\ud to these children. No significant mediation was shown in the later-schooled group.\ud Chapter 6 presents a longitudinal study (n = 45) of the first two years of\ud schooling in a standard school using the cut-off design. Results revealed that a dynamic\ud measure was more effective than a static measure for measuring phoneme awareness in\ud young children. Finally, there was an effect of both age and schooling on the\ud development of phoneme awareness and early reading and spelling skills.\ud These studies demonstrate that age-related factors in addition to schooling play a\ud significant role in the development of reading-related skills. However, although there\ud was evidence of an age effect on literacy skills during the first year of standard\ud schooling (chapter 6), there was only limited evidence of such an effect over a larger age\ud range (chapter 3). In conclusion, it is suggested that concerns that age 4-5 is too early to\ud learn to read are unfounded, and that a delay in school entry age will not necessarily\ud lead to benefits in the acquisition of reading

Topics: BF, LB1501
OAI identifier: oai:wrap.warwick.ac.uk:3901

Suggested articles

Preview

Citations

  1. (1981). A cognitive-developmental theory of reading acquisition. In
  2. (2009). A level results.
  3. (2004). Accelerating the development of reading, spelling and phonemic awareness skills in initial readers. doi
  4. (1999). Age and schooling effects on emergent literacy and early reading skills. doi
  5. (1984). Assessing Phonological Awareness in Kindergarten-Children - doi
  6. (2005). Assessment and early instruction of preschool children at risk for reading disability. doi
  7. (1989). Assisted Assessment - a Taxonomy of Approaches and an Outline of Strengths and Weaknesses. doi
  8. (1998). Awareness of language in children who have reading difficulties: Historical comparisons in a longitudinal study. doi
  9. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In
  10. (2003). Beyond implicit phonological knowledge: No support for an onset-rime structure in children's explicit phonological awareness. doi
  11. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. doi
  12. (1998). Bidirectional relations of phonological sensitivity and prereading abilities: Evidence from a preschool sample. doi
  13. (1996). British Ability Scales: Second edition. Slough: National foundation for educational research.
  14. (1983). Categorizing Sounds and Learning to Read - a Causal Connection. doi
  15. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: A 5-year longitudinal study. doi
  16. (2007). Children at family risk of dyslexia: a follow-up in early adolescence. doi
  17. (1994). Childrens Metalinguistic Knowledge of Syntactic Constituents - doi
  18. (1995). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals 3. San Antonio:
  19. (1982). Common Phoneme and Overall Similarity Relations among Spoken Syllables - Their Use by Children and Adults. doi
  20. (1998). Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading: The role of metalinguistic and short-term memory skills. doi
  21. (2008). Curriculum Guidance for the Early Years Foundation Stage.
  22. (1986). Decoding, reading , and reading disability. doi
  23. (1990). Development of Children with Early Language Delay. doi
  24. (2000). Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latentvariable longitudinal study. doi
  25. (1998). Development of phonological sensitivity in 2- to 5-year-old children. doi
  26. (1994). Development of ReadingRelated Phonological Processing Abilities - New Evidence of Bidirectional Causality from a Latent Variable Longitudinal-Study. doi
  27. (1993). Development of Young Readers Phonological Processing Abilities. doi
  28. (1986). Developmental-Changes in the Cognitive Correlates of Reading-Ability and the Developmental Lag Hypothesis. Reading Research Quarterly, doi
  29. (2000). Developmentally appropriate practice and a National Literacy Strategy. doi
  30. (2007). Develpoment of sight word reading: Phases and findings. doi
  31. (1979). Does Awareness of Speech as a Sequence of Phones Arise Spontaneously. doi
  32. (1988). Does Reading Develop in a Sequence of Stages. doi
  33. (2009). Dynamic Assessment and Response to Intervention Two Sides of One Coin. doi
  34. (1998). Dynamic testing. doi
  35. (1994). Early Learning and a developmentally apropriate curriculum: some lessons from research.
  36. (1995). Education and CognitiveDevelopment - a Natural Experiment.
  37. (1988). Effects of an Extensive Program for Stimulating Phonological Awareness in Preschool-Children. doi
  38. (2003). Effects of orthographic transparency on reading and phoneme awareness in children learning to read in Wales. doi
  39. (2000). Effects of preschool phoneme identity training after six years: Outcome level distinguished from rate of response. doi
  40. (2005). Environmental and genetic influences on prereading skills in Australia, Scandinavia, and the United States. doi
  41. (1991). Evaluation of a Program to Teach Phonemic Awareness to Young-Children. doi
  42. (2009). Exam Results.
  43. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. doi
  44. (1987). Formal Schooling and Task Familiarity - a Reply. doi
  45. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. doi
  46. (2009). Framework for Children’s Learning for 3 to 7-year-olds in Wales.
  47. (2009). from http://www.caci.co.uk/acorn/whatis.asp
  48. (1998). Grapheme-phoneme knowledge is essential for learning to read words in English. In
  49. (1987). How Age-Changes in Knowledge Base Contribute to the Development of Childrens Memory - an Interpretive Review. Developmental Review, doi
  50. (1994). How do season of birth and length of schooling affect children's attainment at key stage 1? doi
  51. (2003). How does orthographic knowledge influence performance on phonological awareness tasks? doi
  52. (1997). How important are rhyme and analogy in beginning reading? doi
  53. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. doi
  54. (2008). Improving early language and literacy skills: differential effects of an 182 oral language versus a phonology with reading intervention. doi
  55. (2006). Independent review of the teaching of early reading: Department for Education and Skills.
  56. (1978). Interaction between learning and development. In
  57. (2009). International thematic probe: The influence of relative age on learner attainment and development:
  58. (1987). Interpreting Data from Illiterates - doi
  59. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? doi
  60. (1995). Joint Book Reading Makes for Success in Learning to Read - a Metaanalysis on Intergenerational Transmission of Literacy. doi
  61. (2006). Knowledge about letters as a foundation for reading and spelling. In
  62. (1996). Knowledge of the alphabet and explicit awareness of phonemes in pre-readers: The nature of the relationship. Reading and Writing, doi
  63. (1984). L'analyse phonetique explicite de la parole et l'acquisition de la lecture [Explicit phonetic analysis of speech and the acquisition of reading]. L'Annee psychologique 84, doi
  64. (1985). Le developpment de l'habilete d'analyse phonetique de la parole [The development of the ability to phonologically analyse speech]. L'Annee psychologique, doi
  65. (1997). Learning a new poem: Memory for connected speech and phonological awareness in low-income children with and without specific language impairment. doi
  66. (2004). Learning a transparent orthography at five years old: reading development of children during their first year of formal reading instruction in Wales. doi
  67. (1984). Leesproblemen en instructiemethoden [Reading problems and instructional method].
  68. (2004). Letter knowledge precipitates phoneme segmentation, but not phoneme invariance. doi
  69. (1986). Matthew Effects in Reading - Some Consequences of Individual-Differences in the Acquisition of Literacy. doi
  70. (1997). Memory for rote linguistic routines and sensitivity to rhyme: A comparison of low-income children with and without specific language impairment. doi
  71. (1992). Metalinguistic development. Chicago: Harvester Wheatsheaf. doi
  72. (1997). Nature-nurture in the classroom: Entrance age, school readiness, and learning in children. doi
  73. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. doi
  74. (2007). Paired-associate learning, phoneme awareness, and learning to read. doi
  75. (1995). Phases of development in learning to read words by sight. doi
  76. (1999). Phases of development in learning to read words. In doi
  77. (2002). Phoneme awareness and pathways to Literacy: A comparison of German and American children.
  78. (2002). Phoneme awareness is a better predictor of early reading skill than onset-rime awareness. doi
  79. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent orthographies: Evidence from Czech and English children. doi
  80. (2005). Phoneme isolation ability is not simply a consequence of letter-sound knowledge. doi
  81. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. doi
  82. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children.
  83. (1990). Phonolgical skills and learning to read. .
  84. (2007). Phonological Abilities Test.
  85. (1991). Phonological Analysis as a Function of Age and Exposure to Reading-Instruction. doi
  86. (1986). Phonological Awareness - the Role of Reading Experience. doi
  87. (1999). Phonological awareness and early reading: A meta-analysis of experimental training studies. doi
  88. (1992). Phonological Memory and Vocabulary Development during the Early School Years - a Longitudinal-Study. Developmental Psychology, doi
  89. (1994). Phonological Sensitivity in Novice Readers and Nonreaders. doi
  90. (2005). Phonological skills are (probably) one cause of success in learning to read: A comment on Castles and Coltheart. Scientific Studies of Reading, doi
  91. (1919). Practical advice to teachers
  92. (2007). Precursors of reading skill from infancy to first grade in Finnish: Continuity and change in a highly inflected language. doi
  93. (2007). Predicting individual differences in learning to read. In doi
  94. (1998). Predicting kindergarten academic skills: Interactions among child care, maternal education, and family literacy environments. doi
  95. (1992). Predicting Progress in Beginning Reading - Dynamic Assessment of Phonemic Awareness. doi
  96. (1999). Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities: Group and individual responses to instruction. doi
  97. (2006). Refining the psycholinguistic grain size theory: effects of phonotactics and word formation on the availability of phonemes to preliterate children. doi
  98. (2007). Relations among socioeconomic status, age, and predictors of phonological awareness. doi
  99. (2002). Relations of the home literacy environment (HLE) to the development of reading-related abilities: A one-year longitudinal study. doi
  100. (1990). Rhyme and Alliteration, Phoneme Detection, and Learning to Read. doi
  101. (2002). Rhyme and reading: a critical review of the research methodology. doi
  102. (1983). Savoir-dire sur la phrase [knowing about sentences].
  103. (2009). School starting age - formal teaching versus play-based learning.
  104. (2001). Schooling and Cognitive development In
  105. (1981). Schooling and the development of cognitive skills. doi
  106. (1997). Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. doi
  107. (1998). Sensitivity to onset and rhyme does predict young children's reading: A comment on Muter, Hulme, Snowling, and Taylor doi
  108. (1990). Social Background, Phonological Awareness and Childrens Reading. doi
  109. (1995). Socioeconomic-Status Differences in Preschool Phonological Sensitivity and First-Grade Reading-Achievement. doi
  110. (1935). Souberman (Eds.), Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes doi
  111. (2007). Sounds: Principles and practice of high quality phonics.
  112. (2000). Specificity in the nature and timing of cognitive growth in Kindergarten and First Grade. doi
  113. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability.
  114. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd edition).
  115. (2005). Steiner schools in England:
  116. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Wasington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.
  117. (2007). Teaching children to read: What do we know about how to do it? doi
  118. (1986). The Ability to Manipulate Speech Sounds Depends on Knowing Alphabetic Writing. doi
  119. (1997). The British Picture Vocabulry Scale. Second Edition.
  120. (2009). The Cambridge Primary Review. Retrieved
  121. (1998). The child's acquisition of the alphabetic principle. doi
  122. (2009). The Cognitive and Linguistic Foundations of Early Reading Development: A Norwegian Latent Variable Longitudinal Study. doi
  123. (2006). The cognitive bases of learning to read and spell in Greek: Evidence from a longitudinal study. doi
  124. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in preschool children. doi
  125. (2001). The effects of global similarity between stimuli on children's judgment of rime and alliteration. doi
  126. (1985). The equilibrium of cognitive structures: The central problem of intellectual development. doi
  127. (2009). The foundation of literacy skills in Korean: the relationship between letter-name knowledge and phonological awareness and their relative contribution to literacy skills. Reading and Writing, doi
  128. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. doi
  129. (2001). The relationship between paired associate learning and phonological skills in normally developing readers. doi
  130. (2005). The relationship between phonological awareness and reading: Implications for the assessment of phonological awareness. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, doi
  131. (1994). The role of home literacy environment in the development of language ability in preschools children from low income families. doi
  132. (1993). The Role of Vocabulary Development in Childrens Spoken Word Recognition and Segmentation Ability. doi
  133. (2009). The Science of Reading: A Handbook. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. doi
  134. (1997). The unique contribution of home literacy environment to differences in early literacy skills. Early Child Development and Care, doi
  135. (2007). Using Multivariate Statistics.
  136. (2008). Variability in children early reading strategies. doi
  137. (2003). Walking with Rosie: A cautionary tale of early reading instruction.
  138. (2002). What are the Educational Experiences of 4-Year-Olds? A comparative study of 4-year-olds in nursery and reception settings. doi
  139. (2009). York assessment of reading for comprehension: GL Assessment.
  140. (1999). Young children's phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. doi

To submit an update or takedown request for this paper, please submit an Update/Correction/Removal Request.