Skip to main content
Article thumbnail
Location of Repository

A detailed analysis of the wholist-analytic style ratio : a methodology for developing a reliable and valid measure of style

By Joanne Davies

Abstract

Riding's (1991) wholist-analytic dimension of cognitive style proposes a unidimensional view of global-analytic constructs, however, very little empirical evidence exists in support of a relationship between the styles in the wholist-analytic family, which has led to suggestions that style is best conceptualised as a more complex multidimensional construct (Hodgkinson and Sadler-Smith, 2003). Another major problem for Riding's (1991) wholist-analytic style construct is its lack of temporal reliability (Peterson, Deary and Austin, 2003; Rezaei and Katz, 2004; Parkinson, Mullally and Redmond, 2004; Cook, 2008). Furthermore, the current thesis argues that in addition to problems of reliability, the wholist-analytic dimension lacks predictive and construct validity. This thesis outlines two major methodological limitations with the current wholist-analytic ratio measurement, which have raised doubts over the efficacy of the ratio in discriminating between part processing and whole processing style. Firstly, the wholist-analytic ratio is confounded by reflective-impulsive style differences (Kagan, Rosman, Day, Albert and Phillips, 1964). Secondly, the nature of the tasks, combined with strategy preferences, set up an asymmetry in the basis of the wholist-analytic ratio. A new measure of wholist-analytic style, hereafter called the 'Wholist-Analytic Style (WAS) Analysis' has been developed to experimentally manipulate the presentation order of the subtests and the number of parts in the geometric stimuli. Performances on the WAS analysis and the CSA were compared to other styles in the wholist-analytic family to test the unidimensional approach to style. It was found that the wholist-analytic ratio is confounded by sensitivity to reflective style, with much of its discriminatory power being limited to the first subtest, and 6 there is an asymmetry in the part-whole processing basis of the wholist-analytic ratio. Furthermore, there is a consistent relationship between reflective-impulsive style and part-whole processing. This thesis proposes the theory of diminished reflection, which renders the wholist-analytic ratio invalid in its current form. The theory can account for the hereto-unexplained lack of temporal reliability of the wholist-analytic ratio and offers a practical solution to improve both the validity and stability of the ratio. This thesis offers partial support for the unidimensional perspective of style but makes strong links between reflective-impulsive style and part-whole processing preferences

Topics: 153.42, Cognitive styles, Cognition
Publisher: University of Glamorgan
Year: 2009
OAI identifier: oai:dspace1.isd.glam.ac.uk:10265/457
Provided by: Glamorgan Dspace

Suggested articles

Citations

  1. (1972). A fresh look at cognition and the individual. doi
  2. (1963). A modification of the embedded figures test for use with young children. doi
  3. (1997). A Real Person: Life on the Outside,
  4. (2002). A.) Consulting Psychologists Press,
  5. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. doi
  6. (2003). An investigation of the interaction between cognitive style and context reinstatement on the memory performance of eyewitnesses. doi
  7. (1983). An Islet of Ability in Autistic-Children - doi
  8. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome. doi
  9. (1981). Aptitudes and instructional methods: a handbook of research on interactions, doi
  10. (1997). Are people with autism or asperger syndrome faster than normal on the embedded figures test. doi
  11. (2006). Autism and family home movies: A comprehensive review. doi
  12. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. doi
  13. (1999). Autism: cognitive deficit or cognitive style? doi
  14. (1989). Autism: Explaining the Enigma, doi
  15. (2007). Celebrating a common finding: Riding's CSA test is unreliable. doi
  16. (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Reading homographs in context. doi
  17. (1987). Cerebral lateralization: biological mechanisms, associations and pathology, MIT press, doi
  18. (1997). Cognitive style and individual differences in EEG alpha during information processing. doi
  19. (1999). Cognitive style and instructional preferences. doi
  20. (1994). Cognitive style and its relevance for management practice. doi
  21. (2000). Cognitive style and motor skill and sports performance. doi
  22. (1998). Cognitive style and problem behaviour in boys referred to residential special schools. doi
  23. (1996). Cognitive style and pupil learning preferences. doi
  24. (1999). Cognitive style and types of problem behaviour in boys in special schools. doi
  25. (1999). Cognitive style, gender and learning from multimedia materials in 11-year-old children. doi
  26. (2003). Cognitive style, working memory and learning behaviour and attainment in school subjects. doi
  27. (1991). Cognitive styles - an overview and intergration. doi
  28. (1991). Cognitive styles analysis: administration, Learning and Training Technology,
  29. (2005). Cognitive styles analysis: personal style awareness, Learning and Training Technology,
  30. (2005). Cognitive styles analysis: style nature and assessment, Learning and Training Technology,
  31. (1964). Cognitive styles and memory for faces. doi
  32. (1972). Cognitive Styles and Responses to Threat of Frustration. doi
  33. (2006). Cognitive styles and student progression in architectural design education. doi
  34. (1972). Cognitive Styles in Adolescents Previously Diagnosed as Hyperactive. doi
  35. (2003). Cognitive styles in the service of language learning. Individual differences: advanced knowledge, doi
  36. (1987). Cognitive styles: an integrated model. doi
  37. (1954). Cognitive stystem-principles of leveling and sharpening: Individual differences in visual time-error assimilation effects. doi
  38. (1977). Cognitive Tempo in Children and Their Parents. Perceptual and Motor Skills, doi
  39. (1977). Cognitive-Style Approach to Creative Thought. doi
  40. (2003). Complex or unitary? A critique and empirical re-assessment of the Allinson-Hayes Cognitive Style Index. doi
  41. (2008). Conceptual versus experimental creativity: Which works best on convergent and divergent thinking tasks. doi
  42. (1987). Construct-Validity of the Childrens Embedded Figures Test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, doi
  43. (1971). Creativity as related to field independence and mobility. doi
  44. (2007). Creativity: Theories and themes: Research, development and practice, doi
  45. (2002). Defining and quantifying the social phenotype in autism. doi
  46. (1978). Development of a more reliable version of the matching familiar figures test. doi
  47. (1993). Development of Forward and Feedback Connections between Areas V1 and V2 of Human Visual-Cortex. doi
  48. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. doi
  49. (1999). EEG differences and cognitive style. doi
  50. (1976). Effects of Locus of Control and Field Independence-Dependence on Moral Reasoning. doi
  51. (2008). Emotional competence in children with autism: Diagnostic criteria and empirical evidence. doi
  52. (1970). Essentials of Psychological Testing, Harper and Row, doi
  53. (2004). Evaluation of the reliability and validity of the cognitive styles analysis. doi
  54. (2003). Examining the self-regulation of impulsive and reflective response styles on academic tasks. doi
  55. (2006). Examining wholistic-analytic style using preferences in early information processing. doi
  56. (2001). Exploring the cognitive phenotype of autism: Weak "central coherence" in parents and siblings of children with autism: I. Experimental tests. doi
  57. (1983). Extraversion, Field-Independence and Performance on Cognitive Tasks
  58. (1994). Face Perception doi
  59. (2001). Face processing occurs outside the fusiform 'face area' in autism: evidence from functional MRI. doi
  60. (1998). Familiar face and voice matching and recognition in children with autism. doi
  61. (1965). Field dependence and external directness. doi
  62. (1972). Field Dependence, Impulsivity, and Learning Disabilities. doi
  63. (1972). Field-dependence and social responsiveness as determinants of spontaneously produced words. Perceptual and Motor Skills, doi
  64. (1977). Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. doi
  65. (2002). Fractionation of visual memory: agency detection and its impairment in autism. doi
  66. (2003). Holistic and part-based face recognition in children with autism. doi
  67. (1983). Humour appreciation as a function of canned laughter, a mirthful companion, and field-dependence: Facilitation and inhibition effects. doi
  68. (1971). In A Manual for the Embedded Figures Tests(Eds, Witkin,
  69. (1971). In Manual for the embedded figures tests(Eds,
  70. (1993). Individual differences in thinking: cognitive and neurphysiological perspective. doi
  71. (2008). Influence of the difficulty of the Matching Familiar Figures Test-20 on the assessment of reflection-impulsivity: An item analysis. doi
  72. (1964). Information processing and the child:significance of analytic and reflective attitudes. doi
  73. (1995). Is the GCSE fair? Examination success and cognitive style.
  74. (1958). Lackland Air Force Base,
  75. (2004). Learning Skills Research
  76. (1975). Longitudinal Development of Formal Operations Thought.
  77. (1973). Moral judgements, behaviour and cognitive style in young children. doi
  78. (2002). Neural correlates of face and object recognition in young children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development. doi
  79. (2003). On the assessment of cognitive style: a commentary on Peterson, Deary, and Austin. Personality and Individual Differences, doi
  80. (2007). On the assessment of the test-retest of riding's CSA: A commentary on doi
  81. (1963). Psychological significance of styles of conceptualization. doi
  82. (2003). Psychometric properties of a Spanish adaptation of the Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFFT-20). doi
  83. (1983). Reading for meaning and reading for sound in autistic and dyslexic children. doi
  84. (2003). Reduced facial muscle movements in Autism: Evidence for dysfunction in the neuromuscular pathway?
  85. (1976). Reflection-Impulsivity - doi
  86. (2001). Reflective-impulsive style and conceptual tempo in a gross motor task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, doi
  87. (1975). Relation of Reflection-Impulsivity to Field DependenceIndependence and Internal-External-Control in Children. doi
  88. (1967). Remembering of words by psychotic and subnormal children. doi
  89. (2007). Reward expectation modulates feedback-related negativity and EEG spectra. doi
  90. (2008). Scores from Riding's Cognitive Styles Analysis have poor testretest reliability. Teaching and Learning in doi
  91. (1973). Sex-Differences in Relationship of Cognitive Style and Intellectual Functioning in Disadvantaged PreschoolChildren. doi
  92. (2002). Sharing humour and laughter in autism and Down's syndrome. doi
  93. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. doi
  94. (1998). Socialization factors in the cognitive style and play of young children. doi
  95. (1970). Socioeconomic Background and Cognitive Functioning in Preschool Children. doi
  96. (1976). Some Methodological Problems in Reflection-Impulsivity Research. doi
  97. (2004). Stability of reflective-impulsive style in coincidenceanticipation motor tasks. doi
  98. (1997). Styles of thinking, abilities, and academic performance. doi
  99. (1971). Task and Social Orientation - Study of Field Dependence, Arousal, and Memory for Incidental Material. Perceptual and Motor Skills, doi
  100. (1972). Task Orientation Versus Social Orientation in Young Children and Their Attention to Relevant Social Cues. doi
  101. (1981). Teachers cognitive styles and their educational implications. doi
  102. (2004). Test-retest reliability of Riding's cognitive styles analysis test. doi
  103. (2008). The central coherence account of autism revisited: Evidence from the ComFor study. doi
  104. (1996). The cognitive style index: A measure of intuition-analysis for organizational research. doi
  105. (1992). The Cognitive Styles of Reflection Impulsivity and Field Independence Dependence and Esl Success. doi
  106. (1997). The effect of cognitive style and cognitive skills on school subject performance. doi
  107. (1997). The effect of cognitive style on the preffed format of instructional material. doi
  108. (1998). The effect of cognitive style, age, gender and structure on the recall of prose passages. doi
  109. (1993). The Effect of Cognitive-Style and Mode of Presentation on Learning-Performance. doi
  110. (2000). The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour -Changes in cerebral blood flow when people with autistic disorder process facial expressions. doi
  111. (1994). The relationship between cognitive style and intelligence. doi
  112. (2003). The reliability of Riding's Cognitive Style Analysis Test. Personality and Individual Differences, doi
  113. (1956). The structure of intellect.
  114. (1996). Theory of mind in the brain: Evidence from a PET scan study of Aspergers syndrome. doi
  115. (2003). Types of task, types of audience, types of actor: interactions between mere presence and personality type in a simple mathematical task. Personality and Individual Differences, doi
  116. (2003). Understanding autism: insights from mind and brain. doi
  117. (2006). Wholist-analytic cognitive style: A matter of reflection. doi

To submit an update or takedown request for this paper, please submit an Update/Correction/Removal Request.