Skip to main content
Article thumbnail
Location of Repository

Anomalies in real and counterfactual worlds: An eye-movement investigation

By Heather J. Ferguson and Anthony J. Sanford

Abstract

Counterfactual reasoning is valid reasoning arising from premises that are true in a hypothetical model, but false in actuality. Investigations of counterfactuals have concentrated on reasoning and production, but psycholinguistic research has been more limited. We report three eye-movement studies investigating the comprehension of counterfactual\ud information. Prior context depicted a counterfactual world (CW), or real world (RW), while a second sentence was manipulated to create RW anomalous continuations, where events included a violation of RW knowledge, and RW\ud congruent continuations, where the events described were congruent with RW knowledge. Results showed that RW violations can be ‘neutralised’ within an appropriate pre-specified CW context, and RW congruent items can lead to\ud the experience of an anomaly following an inconsistent CW context. Importantly, there was also evidence in all three studies for early processing difficulty with RW violations regardless of prior context, indicating that a proposition is rapidly evaluated against real-world knowledge, just prior to the accommodation of a proposition into a counterfactual world representation. We discuss the results in terms of a variety of accounts of the nature of counterfactual worlds

Topics: BF
Year: 2008
OAI identifier: oai:kar.kent.ac.uk:23060

Suggested articles

Citations

  1. (2002). and conditionals: A critique of the mental model theory of Johnson-Laird and Byrne
  2. (1931). Anomalous strings: Eye movement patterns.
  3. (1990). Auditory and visual semantic priming in lexical decision: A comparison using event-related brain potentials.
  4. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma.
  5. (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind on autism: Reading homographs in context.
  6. (1997). Cognitive processes in counterfactual thinking about what might have been. The psychology of learning and motivation,
  7. (1995). Conditional reasoning with false premises: Fantasy and information retrieval.
  8. (2002). Conditionals: A theory of meaning, pragmatics, and inference.
  9. (1984). Conterfactuals based on real possible worlds.
  10. (2003). Context effects in word recognition: Evidence for early interactive processing.
  11. (1989). Coping with novelty in human intelligence—an empirical—investigation.
  12. Counterfactual and semifactual conditionals prime alternative possibilities.
  13. (2001). Counterfactuals in a dynamic context. In Michael Kenstowicz (Ed.), Ken Hale: A life in language.
  14. (1999). Deductive reasoning with factual, possible, and counterfactual conditionals.
  15. (1996). Effects of contextual constraint on eye movements in reading: A further examination.
  16. (1994). Event-related potential components reflect phonological and semantic processing of the terminal word of spoken sentences.
  17. (2004). Evidence for self-organized sentence processing: Digging-in effects.
  18. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research.
  19. (1999). Grammatical aspect in English.
  20. (2000). I couldn’t have known’: Accountability, foreseeability and counterfactual denials of responsibility.
  21. (1999). If pigs could fly’: A test of counterfactual reasoning and pretence in children with autism.
  22. (1999). Incrementality in discourse understanding. In
  23. (1979). Instantiation of verbs.
  24. (1996). Journal of Memory and Language xxx (2007) xxx–xxx 17 ARTICLE IN PRESSReferences
  25. (1997). Mappings in thought and language.
  26. (2005). Memory-based approaches and beyond.
  27. (2002). Mental models and counterfactual thoughts about what might have been.
  28. (1983). Mental models.
  29. (1985). Mental spaces: Aspects of meaning construction in natural language.
  30. (1994). Mental spaces.
  31. (1991). Modality. In A. von Stechow &
  32. (1986). Norm theory— Comparing reality to its alternatives.
  33. (1981). Ordering semantics and premise semantics for counterfactuals.
  34. (1991). Possible worlds and accessibility relations: A semantic typology of fiction.
  35. (2004). Processing discourse roles in scripted narratives: The influences of context and world knowledge.
  36. (2006). Psycholinguistics electrified II: 1994–2005.
  37. (2002). Readers’ eye movements distinguish anomalies of form and content.
  38. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic anomaly.
  39. (1932). Remembering.
  40. (2002). Retrieval of concepts in script-based texts and narratives: The influence of general world knowledge.
  41. (1979). Scripts in memory for text.
  42. (1989). Selection mechanisms in reading lexically ambiguous words.
  43. (1999). Semantic integration in sentences and discourse: Evidence from the N400.
  44. (1987). The complexity of contextual facilitation in reading: Local and global influences.
  45. (2000). The contribution of lexical and situational knowledge to resolving discourse roles:Please cite this article in press as:
  46. (2004). The effects of plausibility on eye movements in reading.
  47. (1983). The mind of man.
  48. (2007). The rational imagination: How people create alternatives to reality.
  49. (1998). The role of scenario mapping in text comprehension.
  50. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In
  51. (2003). The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities.
  52. (2000). Tulip fever.
  53. (1981). Understanding written language.
  54. (1995). Varieties of counterfactual thinking. In
  55. (1999). What are mental models made of. In G. Rickheit & C. Habel (Eds.), MentalPlease cite this article in press as:
  56. (1994). What is who violating? A reconsideration of linguistic violations in light of eventrelated brain potentials. In
  57. (2006). When peanuts fall in love: N400 evidence for the power of discourse.

To submit an update or takedown request for this paper, please submit an Update/Correction/Removal Request.